首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Personality最新文献

英文 中文
Will the Dark Triad Engender Psychopathological Symptoms or Vice Versa? A Three-Wave Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis. 黑暗三合会导致精神病理症状还是相反?三波随机截距交叉滞后面板分析。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12974
Mengpei Wei,Jingguang Li,Xingbo Wang,Zhenglian Su,Yu L L Luo
INTRODUCTIONThe Dark Triad (DT), including narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy, represents the dark side of human nature and has been related to psychopathological symptoms (e.g., depression, anxiety, and stress). However, little is known about how the two constructs are related longitudinally. To fill this gap and to clarify the directionality between them, we conducted a longitudinal study.METHODSWe measured DT traits and psychopathological symptoms in a large sample of university students (NT1 = 1815) annually for 3 years. We implemented random intercept cross-lagged panel models in analysis.RESULTSNarcissism and psychopathological symptoms showed a reciprocal relationship at the within-person level: greater narcissism preceded a decline in psychopathological symptoms, while more severe symptoms preceded a decrease in narcissism. Within the same individual, increases in the DT, particularly psychopathy and Machiavellianism, were linked to concurrent escalations in the symptoms. Additionally, all DT traits were positively correlated with psychopathological symptoms as stable differences between individuals.CONCLUSIONSThis study constitutes an important step in clarifying the directionality between the DT and psychopathological symptoms, and advances our understanding of the interplay between these two constructs at both the between-person and within-person levels.
引言 黑暗三联征(DT)包括自恋、马基雅维利主义和心理变态,代表了人性的阴暗面,与精神病理学症状(如抑郁、焦虑和压力)有关。然而,人们对这两个概念之间的纵向关系知之甚少。为了填补这一空白并明确两者之间的方向性,我们进行了一项纵向研究。方法:我们对一个大样本的大学生(NT1 = 1815)进行了为期 3 年的 DT 特质和精神病理症状的年度测量。结果自恋与精神病理症状在个体内部水平上呈现出互为因果的关系:自恋程度越高,精神病理症状越少;症状越严重,自恋程度越低。在同一个人身上,DT(尤其是心理变态和马基雅维利主义)的增加与症状的同时升级有关。此外,所有 DT 特质都与精神病理症状呈正相关,是个体间的稳定差异。结论:这项研究在澄清 DT 与精神病理症状之间的方向性方面迈出了重要的一步,并加深了我们对这两个概念在人与人之间和人与人之间的相互作用的理解。
{"title":"Will the Dark Triad Engender Psychopathological Symptoms or Vice Versa? A Three-Wave Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis.","authors":"Mengpei Wei,Jingguang Li,Xingbo Wang,Zhenglian Su,Yu L L Luo","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12974","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12974","url":null,"abstract":"INTRODUCTIONThe Dark Triad (DT), including narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy, represents the dark side of human nature and has been related to psychopathological symptoms (e.g., depression, anxiety, and stress). However, little is known about how the two constructs are related longitudinally. To fill this gap and to clarify the directionality between them, we conducted a longitudinal study.METHODSWe measured DT traits and psychopathological symptoms in a large sample of university students (NT1 = 1815) annually for 3 years. We implemented random intercept cross-lagged panel models in analysis.RESULTSNarcissism and psychopathological symptoms showed a reciprocal relationship at the within-person level: greater narcissism preceded a decline in psychopathological symptoms, while more severe symptoms preceded a decrease in narcissism. Within the same individual, increases in the DT, particularly psychopathy and Machiavellianism, were linked to concurrent escalations in the symptoms. Additionally, all DT traits were positively correlated with psychopathological symptoms as stable differences between individuals.CONCLUSIONSThis study constitutes an important step in clarifying the directionality between the DT and psychopathological symptoms, and advances our understanding of the interplay between these two constructs at both the between-person and within-person levels.","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"216 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142321036","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Constructing the "Family Personality": Can Family Functioning Be Linked to Parent-Child Interpersonal Neural Synchronization? 构建 "家庭人格":家庭功能与亲子人际神经同步有关吗?
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12973
Khalil I Thompson, Clayton J Schneider, Joscelin Rocha-Hidalgo, Shri Jeyaram, Bedilia Mata-Centeno, Emily Furtado, Shreeja Vachhani, Koraly Pérez-Edgar, Susan B Perlman

Introduction: Early child development occurs within an interactive environment, initially dominated by parents or caregivers, and is heavily influenced by the dynamics of this social context. The current study probed the neurobiology of "family personality", or family functioning, in the context of parent-child dyadic interaction using a two-person neuroimaging modality.

Methods: One hundred and five parent-child dyads (child mean age 5 years 4 months) were recruited. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning was employed to measure neural synchrony while dyads completed a mildly stressful interactive task. Family functioning was measured through the Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scale IV (FACES-IV).

Results: Synchrony during stress was significantly greater than synchrony during both baseline and recovery conditions for all dyads. A significant interaction between neural synchrony in each task condition and familial balanced flexibility was found, such that higher levels of balanced flexibility were associated with greater changes in frontal cortex neural synchrony as dyads progressed through the three task conditions.

Discussion: Parent-child dyads from families who display heightened levels of balanced flexibility are also more flexible in their engagement of neural synchrony when shifting between social conditions. This is one of the first studies to utilize a two-person imaging modality to explore the links between family functioning and interbrain synchrony between parents and their children.

简介儿童的早期发展是在互动环境中进行的,最初由父母或看护人主导,并在很大程度上受到这种社会环境动态的影响。本研究采用双人神经影像学模式,在亲子互动的背景下探究 "家庭个性 "或家庭功能的神经生物学:方法:招募了 105 个亲子二人组(儿童平均年龄为 5 岁 4 个月)。采用功能性近红外光谱(fNIRS)超扫描测量神经同步性,同时双亲完成一项轻度压力的互动任务。家庭功能通过家庭适应性和凝聚力量表IV(FACES-IV)进行测量:结果:对于所有二人组来说,应激时的同步性明显高于基线和恢复条件下的同步性。每种任务条件下的神经同步性与家庭平衡灵活性之间都存在明显的交互作用,因此,随着二人组在三种任务条件下的进展,平衡灵活性水平越高,额叶皮层神经同步性的变化就越大:讨论:来自平衡灵活性水平较高家庭的亲子二人组在不同社会条件下转换时,神经同步性的参与也更灵活。这是利用双人成像模式探索家庭功能与父母和子女之间脑间同步性之间联系的首批研究之一。
{"title":"Constructing the \"Family Personality\": Can Family Functioning Be Linked to Parent-Child Interpersonal Neural Synchronization?","authors":"Khalil I Thompson, Clayton J Schneider, Joscelin Rocha-Hidalgo, Shri Jeyaram, Bedilia Mata-Centeno, Emily Furtado, Shreeja Vachhani, Koraly Pérez-Edgar, Susan B Perlman","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12973","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12973","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Early child development occurs within an interactive environment, initially dominated by parents or caregivers, and is heavily influenced by the dynamics of this social context. The current study probed the neurobiology of \"family personality\", or family functioning, in the context of parent-child dyadic interaction using a two-person neuroimaging modality.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>One hundred and five parent-child dyads (child mean age 5 years 4 months) were recruited. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) hyperscanning was employed to measure neural synchrony while dyads completed a mildly stressful interactive task. Family functioning was measured through the Family Adaptability and Cohesion Scale IV (FACES-IV).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Synchrony during stress was significantly greater than synchrony during both baseline and recovery conditions for all dyads. A significant interaction between neural synchrony in each task condition and familial balanced flexibility was found, such that higher levels of balanced flexibility were associated with greater changes in frontal cortex neural synchrony as dyads progressed through the three task conditions.</p><p><strong>Discussion: </strong>Parent-child dyads from families who display heightened levels of balanced flexibility are also more flexible in their engagement of neural synchrony when shifting between social conditions. This is one of the first studies to utilize a two-person imaging modality to explore the links between family functioning and interbrain synchrony between parents and their children.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142156416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neuroticism is Associated With Greater Affective Variability at High Levels of Momentary Affective Well-Being, but With Lower Affective Variability at Low Levels of Momentary Affective Well-Being. 神经质与瞬间情感幸福感高水平时的情感变异性有关,但与瞬间情感幸福感低水平时的情感变异性有关。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-09-06 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12972
Mario Wenzel, Whitney R Ringwald, Aleksandra Kaurin, Oliver Tüscher, Thomas Kubiak, Aidan G C Wright

Objective: Research challenged the notion that neuroticism correlates with affective variability, suggesting that it may result from statistical artifacts due to the non-normal distribution of negative affect. We aim to advance this line of research by (a) introducing affect balance as a normally distributed measure of affective well-being and (b) examining current affect balance as a moderator of the relationship between neuroticism and affect balance variability.

Method: We meta-analyzed the results of 14 ambulatory assessment datasets (N = 2389 participants, N = 174,423 observations).

Results: We found that while the associations between the mean and affective variability were large for negative affect, they were much smaller for affect balance. Moreover, the association between neuroticism and variability in negative affect was very small, yet medium-sized for affect balance. Importantly, the latter association depended on current affect levels: Participants high relative to low in neuroticism showed stronger subsequent fluctuations in affect balance when currently feeling better than usual, but weaker subsequent fluctuations in (and thus more persistent) affect balance when currently feeling worse than usual.

Conclusion: Increased variability should not be seen as a bad sign but may be a sign that an affective system is changing, which may be adaptive or maladaptive for an individual, depending on the initial state of the system.

研究目的有研究对神经质与情感变异性相关的观点提出了质疑,认为这可能是由于负面情感的非正态分布所导致的统计假象。我们旨在通过以下方法推进这一研究方向:(a)引入情感平衡作为情感幸福感的正态分布测量指标;(b)研究当前情感平衡作为神经质与情感平衡变异性之间关系的调节因素:方法:我们对 14 个动态评估数据集(N = 2389 名参与者,N = 174423 个观测值)的结果进行了元分析:结果:我们发现,虽然消极情绪的平均值与情绪变异性之间的关联很大,但情绪平衡的关联却小得多。此外,神经质与消极情绪变异性之间的关联非常小,而与情绪平衡之间的关联则为中等大小。重要的是,后一种关联取决于当前的情感水平:神经质程度高的参与者与神经质程度低的参与者相比,在当前感觉比平时好时,情感平衡的后续波动会更强,但在当前感觉比平时差时,情感平衡的后续波动会更弱(因此也更持久):变异性的增加不应被视为一个坏兆头,而可能是情感系统正在发生变化的迹象,这对个人来说可能是适应性的,也可能是不适应性的,这取决于系统的初始状态。
{"title":"Neuroticism is Associated With Greater Affective Variability at High Levels of Momentary Affective Well-Being, but With Lower Affective Variability at Low Levels of Momentary Affective Well-Being.","authors":"Mario Wenzel, Whitney R Ringwald, Aleksandra Kaurin, Oliver Tüscher, Thomas Kubiak, Aidan G C Wright","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12972","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12972","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Research challenged the notion that neuroticism correlates with affective variability, suggesting that it may result from statistical artifacts due to the non-normal distribution of negative affect. We aim to advance this line of research by (a) introducing affect balance as a normally distributed measure of affective well-being and (b) examining current affect balance as a moderator of the relationship between neuroticism and affect balance variability.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We meta-analyzed the results of 14 ambulatory assessment datasets (N = 2389 participants, N = 174,423 observations).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found that while the associations between the mean and affective variability were large for negative affect, they were much smaller for affect balance. Moreover, the association between neuroticism and variability in negative affect was very small, yet medium-sized for affect balance. Importantly, the latter association depended on current affect levels: Participants high relative to low in neuroticism showed stronger subsequent fluctuations in affect balance when currently feeling better than usual, but weaker subsequent fluctuations in (and thus more persistent) affect balance when currently feeling worse than usual.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Increased variability should not be seen as a bad sign but may be a sign that an affective system is changing, which may be adaptive or maladaptive for an individual, depending on the initial state of the system.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Connection Between Dark Traits and Emotional Intelligence: A Multistudy Person-Centered Approach. 黑暗特质与情商之间的联系:以人为本的多重研究方法。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12969
Pilar Rico-Bordera, Jose A Piqueras, Victoria Soto-Sanz, Tíscar Rodríguez-Jiménez, Juan C Marzo, Manuel Galán, David Pineda

Objective: Several studies have analyzed the relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EI) and dark personality, but the results are inconclusive. One study tested correlations between traits that may underlie a person-centered profile of "dark EI." Our study aimed to replicate and extend that research, identifying profiles in Spaniards and examining the differences between the profiles based on different variables of interest.

Method: Two studies were designed, the first one (N = 1241) focused on trait EI and the Dark Triad, and the second one expanding the previous (N = 392) focusing on ability EI and the Dark Tetrad.

Results: Three profiles were obtained in both studies: nondark and non-EI profile, dark profile, and average narcissistic and high EI profile. The latter showed the highest self-esteem, strengths, well-being, and civic engagement, and the lowest psychological difficulties and personal distress.

Conclusions: Our results reinforced the findings of the study we replicated, not supporting the presence of dark EI, but also not supporting the presence of high EI with very low levels of dark traits. These results have implications when designing preventive actions to improve emotional education.

研究目的有几项研究分析了情商(EI)与黑暗人格之间的关系,但结果并不确定。一项研究测试了以人为中心的 "深色情商 "特征之间的相关性。我们的研究旨在复制和扩展该研究,确定西班牙人的特征,并根据不同的相关变量检验特征之间的差异:我们设计了两项研究,第一项研究(N = 1241)侧重于特质EI和黑暗三要素,第二项研究是对前一项研究(N = 392)的扩展,侧重于能力EI和黑暗四要素:结果:两项研究都得出了三种特征:非黑暗和非 EI 特征、黑暗特征以及平均自恋和高 EI 特征。后者表现出最高的自尊、优势、幸福感和公民参与度,最低的心理困难和个人痛苦:我们的研究结果加强了我们复制的研究结果,既不支持存在阴暗 EI,也不支持存在高 EI 但阴暗特质水平很低的情况。这些结果对设计改善情感教育的预防行动具有重要意义。
{"title":"The Connection Between Dark Traits and Emotional Intelligence: A Multistudy Person-Centered Approach.","authors":"Pilar Rico-Bordera, Jose A Piqueras, Victoria Soto-Sanz, Tíscar Rodríguez-Jiménez, Juan C Marzo, Manuel Galán, David Pineda","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12969","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12969","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Several studies have analyzed the relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EI) and dark personality, but the results are inconclusive. One study tested correlations between traits that may underlie a person-centered profile of \"dark EI.\" Our study aimed to replicate and extend that research, identifying profiles in Spaniards and examining the differences between the profiles based on different variables of interest.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Two studies were designed, the first one (N = 1241) focused on trait EI and the Dark Triad, and the second one expanding the previous (N = 392) focusing on ability EI and the Dark Tetrad.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Three profiles were obtained in both studies: nondark and non-EI profile, dark profile, and average narcissistic and high EI profile. The latter showed the highest self-esteem, strengths, well-being, and civic engagement, and the lowest psychological difficulties and personal distress.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our results reinforced the findings of the study we replicated, not supporting the presence of dark EI, but also not supporting the presence of high EI with very low levels of dark traits. These results have implications when designing preventive actions to improve emotional education.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001075","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prolonged unemployment is associated with control loss and personal as well as social disengagement 长期失业与失控、个人和社会脱离有关。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12967
Wiktor Soral, Marcin Bukowski, Michał Bilewicz, Aleksandra Cichocka, Karol Lewczuk, Marta Marchlewska, Aleksandra Rabinovitch, Anna Rędzio, Magdalena Skrodzka, Mirosław Kofta

Objective and Background

The need for control is a fundamental human motivation, that when deprived can lead to broad and substantial changes in human behavior. We aimed to assess the consequences of control deprivation in a real-life situation that poses a severe threat to personal control: a prolonged unemployment.

Method

Using a sample N = 1055 of unemployed (n = 748) versus working (n = 307) individuals, we examined predictions derived from two models of reactions to control deprivation: control-regaining and disengagement/withdrawal.

Results and Conclusions

We found that length unemployment is correlated with a psychological state strongly interfering with psychological as well as social functioning. While control-regaining models of responding to lack of control have received virtually no support from our findings, our results provide evidence that long-term unemployed individuals are more disengaged than working individuals. They are more apathetic, less likely to engage in control-regaining efforts and in active forms of construing one's own future.

目的和背景:控制需求是人类的基本动机,一旦被剥夺,会导致人类行为发生广泛而实质性的变化。我们的目的是评估在对个人控制权构成严重威胁的现实生活中控制权被剥夺的后果:长期失业:方法:我们以失业者(n = 748)和在职者(n = 307)共 1055 人为样本,研究了控制权被剥夺后的两种反应模式:控制权重获模式和脱离/退出模式:结果和结论:我们发现,失业时间长短与心理状态相关,会严重影响心理和社会功能。尽管我们的研究结果几乎没有支持对缺乏控制做出反应的控制-重获模型,但我们的研究结果提供了长期失业者比在职者更加脱离社会的证据。他们更加冷漠,不太可能参与重新获得控制权的努力,也不太可能以积极的方式构建自己的未来。
{"title":"Prolonged unemployment is associated with control loss and personal as well as social disengagement","authors":"Wiktor Soral,&nbsp;Marcin Bukowski,&nbsp;Michał Bilewicz,&nbsp;Aleksandra Cichocka,&nbsp;Karol Lewczuk,&nbsp;Marta Marchlewska,&nbsp;Aleksandra Rabinovitch,&nbsp;Anna Rędzio,&nbsp;Magdalena Skrodzka,&nbsp;Mirosław Kofta","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12967","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12967","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective and Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The need for control is a fundamental human motivation, that when deprived can lead to broad and substantial changes in human behavior. We aimed to assess the consequences of control deprivation in a real-life situation that poses a severe threat to personal control: a prolonged unemployment.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Using a sample <i>N</i> = 1055 of unemployed (<i>n</i> = 748) versus working (<i>n</i> = 307) individuals, we examined predictions derived from two models of reactions to control deprivation: control-regaining and disengagement/withdrawal.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results and Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We found that length unemployment is correlated with a psychological state strongly interfering with psychological as well as social functioning. While control-regaining models of responding to lack of control have received virtually no support from our findings, our results provide evidence that long-term unemployed individuals are more disengaged than working individuals. They are more apathetic, less likely to engage in control-regaining efforts and in active forms of construing one's own future.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"92 6","pages":"1704-1725"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12967","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141876408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the robustness of reciprocal associations between personality and religiosity in a German sample 德国样本中人格与宗教信仰之间相互关联的稳健性。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12964
Richard E. Lucas, Julia M. Rohrer

Objective

Entringer et al. used longitudinal data from a German panel study to examine reciprocal causal effects between personality and religiosity, along with cultural moderators of these effects. The current paper examines the robustness of the original effects to alternative model specifications.

Method

We reanalyzed the same four-wave data spanning 12 years (total N = 46,316), first replicating the original cross-lagged panel analyses and then extending these analyses in three ways: Using a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model, using observed rather than latent variables, and modeling each trait individually rather than simultaneously.

Results

Correlations between personality and religiosity were all small in size, even when aggregating over 12 years. Lagged effects were very small, and none was robust across all model specifications. Cultural moderators also depended on model specifications.

Conclusions

The very small size of these reciprocal effects, along with their sensitivity to model specifications, suggest that conclusions about causal effects of personality and religiosity should be drawn very cautiously.

目的Entringer等人利用德国面板研究的纵向数据,研究了人格与宗教信仰之间的互为因果效应,以及这些效应的文化调节因素。本文研究了原始效应对其他模型规范的稳健性:我们重新分析了同样的跨越 12 年的四波数据(总人数 = 46,316 人),首先复制了最初的交叉滞后面板分析,然后从三个方面扩展了这些分析:使用随机截距交叉滞后面板模型,使用观察变量而非潜在变量,以及对每个特质单独建模而非同时建模:结果:人格与宗教信仰之间的相关性都很小,即使将 12 年的相关性汇总也是如此。滞后效应非常小,而且在所有模型中都不稳健。文化调节因素也取决于模型规格:结论:这些相互影响的规模非常小,而且对模型规格的敏感性也很高,这表明在得出关于人格和宗教信仰的因果影响的结论时应该非常谨慎。
{"title":"On the robustness of reciprocal associations between personality and religiosity in a German sample","authors":"Richard E. Lucas,&nbsp;Julia M. Rohrer","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12964","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12964","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Entringer et al. used longitudinal data from a German panel study to examine reciprocal causal effects between personality and religiosity, along with cultural moderators of these effects. The current paper examines the robustness of the original effects to alternative model specifications.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We reanalyzed the same four-wave data spanning 12 years (total <i>N</i> = 46,316), first replicating the original cross-lagged panel analyses and then extending these analyses in three ways: Using a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model, using observed rather than latent variables, and modeling each trait individually rather than simultaneously.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Correlations between personality and religiosity were all small in size, even when aggregating over 12 years. Lagged effects were very small, and none was robust across all model specifications. Cultural moderators also depended on model specifications.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The very small size of these reciprocal effects, along with their sensitivity to model specifications, suggest that conclusions about causal effects of personality and religiosity should be drawn very cautiously.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"92 6","pages":"1649-1667"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12964","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856762","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A new measure of regulatory focus: Preventing measurement error by promoting best validation practices. 监管重点的新衡量标准:通过推广最佳验证实践防止测量误差。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12962
Paul T Fuglestad, Sarah Volz, Keven Joyal-Desmarais, Steven W Nydick, Colin G DeYoung, Alexander J Rothman

Objective and background: The goals of this project were to improve our understanding of chronic regulatory focus constructs and to provide researchers with a measure that adequately assesses the constructs, can distinguish individual differences effectively across the range of the constructs, and is appropriate for use in diverse populations.

Method: Employing best practices in construct validation, we developed the International Personality Item Pool Regulatory Focus Scale (IPIP-RFS). Utilizing 14 samples (N = 4867), we established substantive (via expert ratings and regulatory focus literature), structural (via factor analysis, item response theory, and measurement invariance), and external (via convergent, discriminant, and predictive associations) validity.

Results: The IPIP-RFS adequately assesses the constructs of chronic promotion focus and prevention focus, can accurately assess individuals along the continua of the constructs, and is suitable for use among populations that vary in gender, race, and age. Individual differences in promotion focus reflect self-regulation and goal pursuit related to cognitive and behavioral exploration and flexibility (i.e., plasticity), whereas individual differences in prevention focus reflect self-regulation and goal pursuit related to motivational and interpersonal steadiness (i.e., stability).

Conclusions: Promotion and prevention focus are important elements of personality with broad implications for functioning and outcomes in health and other important domains.

目标和背景:本项目的目标是加深我们对慢性调控焦点结构的理解,并为研究人员提供一种能够充分评估这些结构、在结构范围内有效区分个体差异并适用于不同人群的测量方法:方法:我们采用构建验证的最佳实践,开发了国际人格项目库监管焦点量表(IPIP-RFS)。利用 14 个样本(N = 4867),我们建立了实质效度(通过专家评分和调控焦点文献)、结构效度(通过因子分析、项目反应理论和测量不变性)和外部效度(通过收敛性、判别性和预测性关联):IPIP-RFS充分评估了慢性促进焦点和预防焦点的构造,能够沿着构造的连续性对个体进行准确评估,适合在性别、种族和年龄不同的人群中使用。促进重点的个体差异反映了与认知和行为探索及灵活性(即可塑性)相关的自我调节和目标追求,而预防重点的个体差异则反映了与动机和人际稳定性(即稳定性)相关的自我调节和目标追求:结论:促进和预防关注是人格的重要因素,对健康和其他重要领域的功能和结果具有广泛影响。
{"title":"A new measure of regulatory focus: Preventing measurement error by promoting best validation practices.","authors":"Paul T Fuglestad, Sarah Volz, Keven Joyal-Desmarais, Steven W Nydick, Colin G DeYoung, Alexander J Rothman","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12962","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12962","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective and background: </strong>The goals of this project were to improve our understanding of chronic regulatory focus constructs and to provide researchers with a measure that adequately assesses the constructs, can distinguish individual differences effectively across the range of the constructs, and is appropriate for use in diverse populations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Employing best practices in construct validation, we developed the International Personality Item Pool Regulatory Focus Scale (IPIP-RFS). Utilizing 14 samples (N = 4867), we established substantive (via expert ratings and regulatory focus literature), structural (via factor analysis, item response theory, and measurement invariance), and external (via convergent, discriminant, and predictive associations) validity.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The IPIP-RFS adequately assesses the constructs of chronic promotion focus and prevention focus, can accurately assess individuals along the continua of the constructs, and is suitable for use among populations that vary in gender, race, and age. Individual differences in promotion focus reflect self-regulation and goal pursuit related to cognitive and behavioral exploration and flexibility (i.e., plasticity), whereas individual differences in prevention focus reflect self-regulation and goal pursuit related to motivational and interpersonal steadiness (i.e., stability).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Promotion and prevention focus are important elements of personality with broad implications for functioning and outcomes in health and other important domains.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11775232/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789538","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion regulation and self-control: Same same but different… and even incompatible? 情绪调节与自我控制:同中有异......甚至互不相容?
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12965
Mario Wenzel, Zarah Rowland, Sebastian Bürgler, Marie Hennecke

Objective

We explore the idea that inhibitory self-control and prohedonic emotion regulation may be incompatible concerns.

Background

Specifically, we propose that because some forms of self-control involve denying oneself hedonic pleasures, it may lead to negative affect. Because people may then prioritize emotion regulation over self-control, negative affect may in turn lead to emotion regulation efforts, specifically the use of emotion regulation strategies, and an increased likelihood of self-control failure.

Method

To explore the relationship between emotion regulation and self-control in daily life, we conducted a secondary analysis of a 6-week, 6-signal-per-day ambulatory assessment data set (N = 125 participants with a total of 22,845 completed measurement occasions).

Results

Consistent with our predictions, we found that self-control efforts of resisting a pleasurable desire led to significantly increased subsequent negative affect, which, in turn, led to significantly increased emotion regulation efforts and to significantly more likely self-control failures.

Conclusions

We found evidence for the notion that inhibitory self-control and prohedonic emotion regulation are, on average, somewhat incompatible concerns. We discuss our findings in the context of other phenomena in which emotion regulation concerns may conflict with the pursuit of other goals.

目的:我们探讨了抑制性自我控制和顺应性情绪调节可能互不相容的观点:背景:我们探讨了抑制性自我控制和享乐性情绪调节可能互不相容的观点:具体来说,我们提出,由于某些形式的自我控制涉及到拒绝享乐,因此可能会导致负面情绪。由于人们可能会将情绪调节置于自我控制之上,负面情绪可能会反过来导致情绪调节努力,特别是情绪调节策略的使用,并增加自我控制失败的可能性:为了探索日常生活中情绪调节与自我控制之间的关系,我们对为期6周、每天6个信号的动态评估数据集(N = 125名参与者,共完成22,845次测量)进行了二次分析:结果:与我们的预测一致,我们发现抵制快乐欲望的自我控制努力会导致随后的负面情绪显著增加,这反过来又会导致情绪调节努力显著增加,自我控制失败的可能性显著增加:我们发现有证据表明,平均而言,抑制性自我控制和顺从性情绪调节在某种程度上是不相容的。我们结合情绪调节可能与追求其他目标相冲突的其他现象来讨论我们的发现。
{"title":"Emotion regulation and self-control: Same same but different… and even incompatible?","authors":"Mario Wenzel,&nbsp;Zarah Rowland,&nbsp;Sebastian Bürgler,&nbsp;Marie Hennecke","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12965","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12965","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We explore the idea that inhibitory self-control and prohedonic emotion regulation may be incompatible concerns.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Specifically, we propose that because some forms of self-control involve denying oneself hedonic pleasures, it may lead to negative affect. Because people may then prioritize emotion regulation over self-control, negative affect may in turn lead to emotion regulation efforts, specifically the use of emotion regulation strategies, and an increased likelihood of self-control failure.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>To explore the relationship between emotion regulation and self-control in daily life, we conducted a secondary analysis of a 6-week, 6-signal-per-day ambulatory assessment data set (<i>N</i> = 125 participants with a total of 22,845 completed measurement occasions).</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Consistent with our predictions, we found that self-control efforts of resisting a pleasurable desire led to significantly increased subsequent negative affect, which, in turn, led to significantly increased emotion regulation efforts and to significantly more likely self-control failures.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We found evidence for the notion that inhibitory self-control and prohedonic emotion regulation are, on average, somewhat incompatible concerns. We discuss our findings in the context of other phenomena in which emotion regulation concerns may conflict with the pursuit of other goals.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"92 6","pages":"1668-1682"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12965","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using structural topic modeling to understand ethnicity-related narratives 利用结构主题模型理解与种族有关的叙述。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12966
Rachel Jacobson, Dulce Wilkinson Westberg, Edward Chou, Moin Syed, Sara J. Weston

Objective

Narrative identity is a promising approach for understanding the content of individuals' ethnic identities but can be limited by the time-intensive nature of human coding and the reliance on preestablished coding systems.

Background

The aim of our preregistered study is to elucidate the content of individuals' ethnicity-related experiences using a novel statistical approach.

Method

We applied structural topic modeling (STM), a natural language processing tool, to narratives written by an ethnically diverse sample of 1149 young adults about a moment they felt aware of their ethnicity.

Results

We identified 14 topics within ethnicity narratives and analyzed how each topic related to both the participant's ethnicity and the human-coded themes of agency and communion. For example, the topic Gained perspective of structural inequality was associated with greater agency, whereas Peer dynamics was associated with greater communion. Ethnic/cultural celebration was associated with both.

Conclusions

This study introduces STM as a useful tool for extracting topic content in narrative data and demonstrates how the multi-method assessment of ethnicity narratives provides greater insight into the content of ethnic experiences. These findings contribute to our understanding of contextualized aspects of personality, including the innovative ways we might examine them.

目的:叙事身份认同是一种很有前途的了解个人民族身份内容的方法,但可能会受到人类编码的时间密集性和对预设编码系统的依赖性的限制:叙述性身份认同是了解个人种族身份内容的一种有前途的方法,但可能会受到人为编码的时间密集性和对预先建立的编码系统的依赖性的限制:背景:我们预先注册的研究旨在使用一种新颖的统计方法来阐明个人民族相关经历的内容:方法:我们将结构主题建模(STM)这一自然语言处理工具应用于由1149名年轻成年人组成的种族多元化样本所撰写的关于他们意识到自己种族的某一时刻的叙述:结果:我们在种族叙事中确定了 14 个主题,并分析了每个主题与参与者的种族以及人类编码的 "代理 "和 "共融 "主题之间的关系。例如,"获得结构性不平等的视角 "这一主题与更大的能动性相关,而 "同伴动态 "则与更大的共融性相关。种族/文化庆祝活动与这两个主题都有关联:本研究将 STM 介绍为从叙事数据中提取主题内容的有用工具,并展示了对种族叙事进行多方法评估如何更深入地了解种族经历的内容。这些发现有助于我们理解人格的情境化方面,包括我们可能对其进行研究的创新方法。
{"title":"Using structural topic modeling to understand ethnicity-related narratives","authors":"Rachel Jacobson,&nbsp;Dulce Wilkinson Westberg,&nbsp;Edward Chou,&nbsp;Moin Syed,&nbsp;Sara J. Weston","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12966","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12966","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Narrative identity is a promising approach for understanding the content of individuals' ethnic identities but can be limited by the time-intensive nature of human coding and the reliance on preestablished coding systems.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The aim of our preregistered study is to elucidate the content of individuals' ethnicity-related experiences using a novel statistical approach.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We applied structural topic modeling (STM), a natural language processing tool, to narratives written by an ethnically diverse sample of 1149 young adults about a moment they felt aware of their ethnicity.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We identified 14 topics within ethnicity narratives and analyzed how each topic related to both the participant's ethnicity and the human-coded themes of agency and communion. For example, the topic <i>Gained perspective of structural inequality</i> was associated with greater agency, whereas <i>Peer dynamics</i> was associated with greater communion. <i>Ethnic/cultural celebration</i> was associated with both.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study introduces STM as a useful tool for extracting topic content in narrative data and demonstrates how the multi-method assessment of ethnicity narratives provides greater insight into the content of ethnic experiences. These findings contribute to our understanding of contextualized aspects of personality, including the innovative ways we might examine them.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"92 6","pages":"1683-1703"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141749357","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Negative urgency as a state-level process. 作为国家级进程的 "消极紧迫性"。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12961
Kevin M King, Madison C Feil, Nancy Gomez Juarez, Diego Moss, Max A Halvorson, Jonas Dora, Natalie F Upton, Morgan A Bryson, Katherine Seldin, Yuichi Shoda, Christine M Lee, Gregory T Smith

Objective: Test whether global self-reports of urgency moderated the within-person associations of affect and impulsive behaviors.

Background: Negative urgency is a personality trait that is a risk factor for a range of psychopathology. Although it is assumed that global self-reports of urgency measure individual tendencies to act more impulsively in the face of negative emotions, evidence from ecological momentary assessment studies is mixed.

Method: In this Registered Report, we used ecological momentary assessment data from a large sample of young adults (n = 496, age 18-22, 5 surveys per day for 40 days).

Results: All forms of momentary impulsivity were impaired in moments when people reported more intense negative emotions, but global self-reports of urgency did not explain individual differences in this association. Moreover, averaged affective states, rather than specific dimensions, affective circumplex, or appraisals, best predicted impulsive states.

Conclusions: Results suggest that face-valid interpretations of global self-report of urgency are inaccurate, and it may be important to understand how some people come to understand themselves as high on urgency rather than assuming that people's self-reports of their motivations are accurate. Momentary experiences of emotions globally impact multiple weakly to moderately associated impulsive behaviors, and future research should seek to understand both when and for whom these associations are strongest.

目的: 测试全球紧迫感自我报告是否会调节人与人之间的情感和冲动行为:测试紧迫感的总体自我报告是否会调节情感和冲动行为的人际关联:背景:负性紧迫感是一种人格特质,是一系列精神病理学的风险因素。尽管人们认为紧迫感的总体自我报告可以衡量个人在面对负面情绪时的冲动倾向,但来自生态瞬间评估研究的证据却不尽相同:在这份注册报告中,我们使用了大量青少年样本(n = 496,18-22 岁,每天 5 次调查,持续 40 天)的生态瞬间评估数据:结果:当人们报告的负面情绪更强烈时,所有形式的瞬间冲动都会受到影响,但对紧迫性的整体自我报告并不能解释这种关联中的个体差异。此外,平均的情绪状态,而不是特定的维度、情绪包络或评价,最能预测冲动状态:结论:研究结果表明,从表面验证的角度来解释全球自我报告的紧迫性是不准确的,因此,重要的是要了解一些人是如何将自己理解为高紧迫性的,而不是假设人们对其动机的自我报告是准确的。情绪的瞬间体验会对多种弱到中度相关的冲动行为产生全面影响,未来的研究应设法了解这些关联在何时以及对谁的影响最大。
{"title":"Negative urgency as a state-level process.","authors":"Kevin M King, Madison C Feil, Nancy Gomez Juarez, Diego Moss, Max A Halvorson, Jonas Dora, Natalie F Upton, Morgan A Bryson, Katherine Seldin, Yuichi Shoda, Christine M Lee, Gregory T Smith","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12961","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12961","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Test whether global self-reports of urgency moderated the within-person associations of affect and impulsive behaviors.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Negative urgency is a personality trait that is a risk factor for a range of psychopathology. Although it is assumed that global self-reports of urgency measure individual tendencies to act more impulsively in the face of negative emotions, evidence from ecological momentary assessment studies is mixed.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In this Registered Report, we used ecological momentary assessment data from a large sample of young adults (n = 496, age 18-22, 5 surveys per day for 40 days).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>All forms of momentary impulsivity were impaired in moments when people reported more intense negative emotions, but global self-reports of urgency did not explain individual differences in this association. Moreover, averaged affective states, rather than specific dimensions, affective circumplex, or appraisals, best predicted impulsive states.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Results suggest that face-valid interpretations of global self-report of urgency are inaccurate, and it may be important to understand how some people come to understand themselves as high on urgency rather than assuming that people's self-reports of their motivations are accurate. Momentary experiences of emotions globally impact multiple weakly to moderately associated impulsive behaviors, and future research should seek to understand both when and for whom these associations are strongest.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11739430/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141628110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1