首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Personality最新文献

英文 中文
A Meta-Analytic Review of Personality and Teacher-Student Relationships. 人格与师生关系的元分析综述。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12986
Sakhavat Mammadov, Ayse Hilal Avci

Objective: This meta-analysis provides the first comprehensive synthesis of associations between student and teacher personality traits and the quality of teacher-student relationships.

Method: Fifty-five studies met the eligibility criteria, contributing a total of 238 effect sizes. We used multivariate meta-analysis with robust variance estimation (RVE) to model the dependency of effect sizes.

Results: Student prosocial behavior (ρ = 0.59) and proactive personality (ρ = 0.48) were the strongest predictors of positive teacher-student relationship quality. All Big Five traits of students, except neuroticism, showed significant positive associations, with all correlations exceeding 0.4 when holding constant with all other moderators. Teacher agreeableness (ρ = 0.31) and conscientiousness (ρ = 0.29) yielded modest associations. Students with less emotional stability or aggressive behaviors were more likely to experience negative teacher-student relationships, such as conflict and dependency.

Conclusion: Our findings highlight the critical role of student personality in explaining the quality of teacher-student interactions. Multiple traits appear equally important, as indicated by comparable effect sizes. The literature is relatively limited when it comes to teacher personality. We were unable to examine teacher traits in relation to teacher-student conflict and dependency, but, overall, agreeable and conscientious teacher behaviors appeared to be important for favorable interactions, whereas teacher neuroticism may undermine the quality of such interactions.

目的这项荟萃分析首次对学生和教师的人格特质与师生关系质量之间的关联进行了全面综合:方法:55 项研究符合资格标准,共有 238 个效应大小。我们使用多变量荟萃分析法和稳健方差估计法(RVE)对效应大小的依赖性进行建模:学生的亲社会行为(ρ = 0.59)和积极主动的个性(ρ = 0.48)是预测积极师生关系质量的最有力因素。除神经质外,学生的所有大五特质都显示出显著的正相关,在所有其他调节因素不变的情况下,相关系数都超过了 0.4。教师的宜人性(ρ = 0.31)和自觉性(ρ = 0.29)产生了适度的关联。情绪稳定性较差或有攻击性行为的学生更有可能经历消极的师生关系,如冲突和依赖:我们的研究结果凸显了学生个性在解释师生互动质量方面的关键作用。多种特质似乎同样重要,这一点从可比的效应大小可以看出。关于教师个性的文献相对有限。我们无法研究教师特质与师生冲突和依赖性的关系,但总体而言,教师的合意和认真负责的行为似乎对良好的互动很重要,而教师的神经质则可能会破坏这种互动的质量。
{"title":"A Meta-Analytic Review of Personality and Teacher-Student Relationships.","authors":"Sakhavat Mammadov, Ayse Hilal Avci","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12986","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12986","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This meta-analysis provides the first comprehensive synthesis of associations between student and teacher personality traits and the quality of teacher-student relationships.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Fifty-five studies met the eligibility criteria, contributing a total of 238 effect sizes. We used multivariate meta-analysis with robust variance estimation (RVE) to model the dependency of effect sizes.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Student prosocial behavior (ρ = 0.59) and proactive personality (ρ = 0.48) were the strongest predictors of positive teacher-student relationship quality. All Big Five traits of students, except neuroticism, showed significant positive associations, with all correlations exceeding 0.4 when holding constant with all other moderators. Teacher agreeableness (ρ = 0.31) and conscientiousness (ρ = 0.29) yielded modest associations. Students with less emotional stability or aggressive behaviors were more likely to experience negative teacher-student relationships, such as conflict and dependency.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Our findings highlight the critical role of student personality in explaining the quality of teacher-student interactions. Multiple traits appear equally important, as indicated by comparable effect sizes. The literature is relatively limited when it comes to teacher personality. We were unable to examine teacher traits in relation to teacher-student conflict and dependency, but, overall, agreeable and conscientious teacher behaviors appeared to be important for favorable interactions, whereas teacher neuroticism may undermine the quality of such interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142523389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Posttraumatic Growth: The Role of Health and Financial Difficulties During a Pandemic. 创伤后成长:大流行病期间健康和经济困难的作用。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12981
Rachel S Rauvola, Cort W Rudolph, Hannes Zacher

Objective: Trauma can have a range of effects on individuals over time, including the potential for positive changes in favorable outcomes commonly referred to as posttraumatic growth. The posttraumatic growth literature has been criticized for various methodological limitations and has largely neglected the exploration of factors that may strengthen or weaken posttraumatic growth trajectories. The present study contributes to this literature by investigating trajectories of five dimensions of posttraumatic growth during the COVID-19 pandemic and the moderating effects of health and financial difficulties on these trajectories.

Method: Longitudinal data were collected monthly between July 2020 and December 2022 from a sample of employed adults in Germany, involving N = 1678 participants over 30 time points (Nobservations = 29,552).

Results: Results suggest that certain dimensions of posttraumatic growth showed significant increases (i.e., relationships) or decreases (i.e., spirituality and possibilities) over time and that trajectories of three dimensions (i.e., relationships, appreciation, and strength) demonstrated distinctive patterns of interaction with health and financial difficulties.

Conclusions: Findings are discussed in the context of the pandemic as well as dynamic theories of personality and identity, with implications for advancing posttraumatic growth theory and research.

目的:随着时间的推移,创伤会对个人产生一系列影响,其中包括可能出现积极变化的有利结果,这通常被称为创伤后成长。创伤后成长的文献因各种方法上的局限性而饱受批评,并且在很大程度上忽视了对可能加强或削弱创伤后成长轨迹的因素的探讨。本研究通过调查 COVID-19 大流行期间创伤后成长五个维度的轨迹,以及健康和经济困难对这些轨迹的调节作用,为这一文献做出了贡献:方法:在2020年7月至2022年12月期间,每月从德国的就业成年人样本中收集纵向数据,在30个时间点上涉及N = 1678名参与者(Nobservations = 29,552):结果表明,随着时间的推移,创伤后成长的某些维度(即人际关系)出现了显著增加或减少(即精神性和可能性),三个维度(即人际关系、欣赏和力量)的轨迹与健康和经济困难之间呈现出独特的互动模式:结论:研究结果在大流行病以及人格和身份动态理论的背景下进行了讨论,对推动创伤后成长理论和研究具有重要意义。
{"title":"Posttraumatic Growth: The Role of Health and Financial Difficulties During a Pandemic.","authors":"Rachel S Rauvola, Cort W Rudolph, Hannes Zacher","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12981","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12981","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Trauma can have a range of effects on individuals over time, including the potential for positive changes in favorable outcomes commonly referred to as posttraumatic growth. The posttraumatic growth literature has been criticized for various methodological limitations and has largely neglected the exploration of factors that may strengthen or weaken posttraumatic growth trajectories. The present study contributes to this literature by investigating trajectories of five dimensions of posttraumatic growth during the COVID-19 pandemic and the moderating effects of health and financial difficulties on these trajectories.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Longitudinal data were collected monthly between July 2020 and December 2022 from a sample of employed adults in Germany, involving N = 1678 participants over 30 time points (N<sub>observations</sub> = 29,552).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results suggest that certain dimensions of posttraumatic growth showed significant increases (i.e., relationships) or decreases (i.e., spirituality and possibilities) over time and that trajectories of three dimensions (i.e., relationships, appreciation, and strength) demonstrated distinctive patterns of interaction with health and financial difficulties.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings are discussed in the context of the pandemic as well as dynamic theories of personality and identity, with implications for advancing posttraumatic growth theory and research.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142523390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personal Relative Deprivation and Locus of Control. 个人相对剥夺感和控制感。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-22 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12980
Mitchell J Callan, Robbie M Sutton, Phatthanakit Chobthamkit, Victoria Wai Lan Yeung, Florence Y N Leung, Ryosuke Asano, Peter Beattie, Allan B I Bernardo, Chinun Boonroungrut, Jen-Ho Chang, Anindita Chaudhuri, Chin-Lung Chien, Hoon-Seok Choi, Lixian Cui, Hongfei Du, Alexander Scott English, Kei Fuji, Hidefumi Hitokoto, Junko Iida, Keiko Ishii, Ding-Yu Jiang, Yashpal Jogdand, Hyejoo J Lee, Nobuhiro Mifune, Aya Murayama, Jinkyung Na, Kim One, Joonha Park, Kosuke Sato, Punit Shah, Suryodaya Sharma, Eunkook M Suh, Arun Tipandjan, Michael Shengtao Wu, William J Skylark

Objective: We investigated the relationship between personal relative deprivation (PRD)-resentment from the belief that one is worse off than people who are similar to oneself-and locus of control.

Background: Research has yet to comprehensively investigate whether PRD is associated with a tendency to favor external (vs. internal) explanations for self- and other-relevant outcomes.

Method: Eight studies (Ntotal = 6729) employed cross-sectional, experimental, and (micro)longitudinal designs and used established trait and state measures of PRD and loci of control.

Results: Participants higher in PRD adopted more external (vs. internal) explanations for others' outcomes while controlling for socio-demographics (e.g., socioeconomic status; Studies 1-4). This relationship was mediated by a lowered sense of personal control (Study 1) and evident in a cross-national sample of participants in Asia (Study 2). PRD is more robustly associated with external than internal explanations for self and other-relevant outcomes (Studies 5-8), and within-person changes in PRD are positively associated with within-person changes in external explanations (month-to-month and day-to-day; Studies 7-8).

Conclusions: PRD is positively associated with external locus of control independent of socioeconomic status, within and between people, and across cultures. This research highlights the implications of PRD for people's construal of the causal forces that govern their lives.

目的:我们研究了个人相对剥夺感(PRD)--认为自己比与自己相似的人生活得差而产生的怨恨--与控制感之间的关系:我们研究了个人相对剥夺感(PRD)--认为自己比与自己相似的人生活得差而产生的怨恨--与控制感之间的关系:背景:关于个人相对剥夺(PRD)是否与倾向于对自我和他人相关结果进行外部(或内部)解释有关,目前还没有全面的研究:八项研究(总人数 = 6729)采用了横截面、实验和(微观)纵向设计,并使用了已确立的 PRD 和控制位置的特质和状态测量方法:结果:在控制社会人口统计学因素(如社会经济地位;研究 1-4)的情况下,PRD 较高的参与者对他人的结果采用了更多的外部(与内部)解释。这种关系是由个人控制感的降低(研究 1)和亚洲参与者的跨国样本(研究 2)所促成的。PRD与自我和他人相关结果的外部解释相比,与内部解释的相关性更强(研究5-8),PRD的个人内部变化与外部解释的个人内部变化(逐月和逐日;研究7-8)呈正相关:结论:PRD 与外部控制力呈正相关,不受社会经济地位的影响,在人与人之间以及不同文化中都是如此。这项研究强调了PRD对人们理解支配其生活的因果力量的影响。
{"title":"Personal Relative Deprivation and Locus of Control.","authors":"Mitchell J Callan, Robbie M Sutton, Phatthanakit Chobthamkit, Victoria Wai Lan Yeung, Florence Y N Leung, Ryosuke Asano, Peter Beattie, Allan B I Bernardo, Chinun Boonroungrut, Jen-Ho Chang, Anindita Chaudhuri, Chin-Lung Chien, Hoon-Seok Choi, Lixian Cui, Hongfei Du, Alexander Scott English, Kei Fuji, Hidefumi Hitokoto, Junko Iida, Keiko Ishii, Ding-Yu Jiang, Yashpal Jogdand, Hyejoo J Lee, Nobuhiro Mifune, Aya Murayama, Jinkyung Na, Kim One, Joonha Park, Kosuke Sato, Punit Shah, Suryodaya Sharma, Eunkook M Suh, Arun Tipandjan, Michael Shengtao Wu, William J Skylark","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12980","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12980","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We investigated the relationship between personal relative deprivation (PRD)-resentment from the belief that one is worse off than people who are similar to oneself-and locus of control.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Research has yet to comprehensively investigate whether PRD is associated with a tendency to favor external (vs. internal) explanations for self- and other-relevant outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Eight studies (N<sub>total</sub> = 6729) employed cross-sectional, experimental, and (micro)longitudinal designs and used established trait and state measures of PRD and loci of control.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants higher in PRD adopted more external (vs. internal) explanations for others' outcomes while controlling for socio-demographics (e.g., socioeconomic status; Studies 1-4). This relationship was mediated by a lowered sense of personal control (Study 1) and evident in a cross-national sample of participants in Asia (Study 2). PRD is more robustly associated with external than internal explanations for self and other-relevant outcomes (Studies 5-8), and within-person changes in PRD are positively associated with within-person changes in external explanations (month-to-month and day-to-day; Studies 7-8).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PRD is positively associated with external locus of control independent of socioeconomic status, within and between people, and across cultures. This research highlights the implications of PRD for people's construal of the causal forces that govern their lives.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Just Because I'm Great (and You're Not): When, Why, and How Narcissistic Individuals Give Gifts to Others 只因为我很棒(而你不是):自恋者何时、为何以及如何向他人赠送礼物
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-19 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12983
Colleen P. Kirk, Constantine Sedikides, Julian Givi
ObjectiveWe examined the roles of Narcissistic Admiration and Narcissistic Rivalry in gift giving. We hypothesized that Admirative and Rivalrous individuals diverge in their likelihood of giving gifts.MethodAcross six studies (ΣN = 2198), we used correlational and experimental methodology and capitalized on both scenarios and actual gift giving.ResultsNarcissistic Admiration was positively, but Narcissistic Rivalry was negatively, associated with gift‐giving likelihood (Studies 1–2). These findings were explained by diverging communal motivations for gift giving (Study 3). Consistent with the notion that Rivalrous individuals are less likely to give gifts for communal reasons because they feel threatened by social closeness, the negative association between Narcissistic Rivalry and gift‐giving likelihood was attenuated when the gift recipient was more socially distant (vs. close; Study 4). Further, gifts that are recipient‐centric (e.g., customized with a recipient's name) are less focused on attributes of the giver and less likely to foster social closeness. Therefore, consistent with Admirative individuals' use of gift giving to promote themselves as a superior communal relationship partner, the positive association between Narcissistic Admiration and gift‐giving likelihood was attenuated for gifts that were recipient‐centric (Study 5). Socially desirable responding, self‐esteem, and fear of failure (Study SM1) did not account for the findings.
目的我们研究了自恋型仰慕和自恋型竞争在赠送礼物中的作用。我们假设,自恋型仰慕者和自恋型竞争者在赠送礼品的可能性上存在差异。方法在六项研究(ΣN = 2198)中,我们采用了相关和实验方法,并利用了情景和实际赠送礼品的情况。结果自恋型仰慕者与赠送礼品的可能性呈正相关,而自恋型竞争者与赠送礼品的可能性呈负相关(研究 1-2)。送礼的共同动机不同可以解释这些发现(研究 3)。与 "竞争型 "个体因感到社会亲近感的威胁而不太可能出于公共原因送礼这一观点相一致的是,当收礼人的社会关系更疏远时(相对于更亲近;研究 4),"自恋型竞争 "与送礼可能性之间的负相关就会减弱。此外,以受礼者为中心的礼物(例如,以受礼者的名字定制的礼物)不太注重送礼者的特质,也不太可能促进社会亲近感。因此,自恋型仰慕者通过赠送礼物将自己提升为优秀的社交关系伙伴,而以收礼者为中心的礼物则削弱了自恋型仰慕者与赠送礼物可能性之间的正相关(研究 5),这与自恋型仰慕者通过赠送礼物将自己提升为优秀的社交关系伙伴是一致的。社会期望反应、自尊和对失败的恐惧(研究 SM1)并不能解释这些发现。
{"title":"Just Because I'm Great (and You're Not): When, Why, and How Narcissistic Individuals Give Gifts to Others","authors":"Colleen P. Kirk, Constantine Sedikides, Julian Givi","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12983","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12983","url":null,"abstract":"ObjectiveWe examined the roles of Narcissistic Admiration and Narcissistic Rivalry in gift giving. We hypothesized that Admirative and Rivalrous individuals diverge in their likelihood of giving gifts.MethodAcross six studies (Σ<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 2198), we used correlational and experimental methodology and capitalized on both scenarios and actual gift giving.ResultsNarcissistic Admiration was positively, but Narcissistic Rivalry was negatively, associated with gift‐giving likelihood (Studies 1–2). These findings were explained by diverging communal motivations for gift giving (Study 3). Consistent with the notion that Rivalrous individuals are less likely to give gifts for communal reasons because they feel threatened by social closeness, the negative association between Narcissistic Rivalry and gift‐giving likelihood was attenuated when the gift recipient was more socially distant (vs. close; Study 4). Further, gifts that are recipient‐centric (e.g., customized with a recipient's name) are less focused on attributes of the giver and less likely to foster social closeness. Therefore, consistent with Admirative individuals' use of gift giving to promote themselves as a superior communal relationship partner, the positive association between Narcissistic Admiration and gift‐giving likelihood was attenuated for gifts that were recipient‐centric (Study 5). Socially desirable responding, self‐esteem, and fear of failure (Study SM1) did not account for the findings.","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"83 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142451390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Everyday State Attachment: Dynamic Features and Role of Trait Attachment 日常状态依恋:特质依恋的动态特征和作用
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12975
Jaakko Tammilehto, Aleksandra Kaurin, Guy Bosmans, Peter Kuppens, Marjo Flykt, Mervi Vänskä, Kirsi Peltonen, Jallu Lindblom
ObjectiveAttachment research has traditionally focused on traits, enhancing our understanding of attachment‐related individual differences. However, to chart the dynamic properties of the attachment system, more research is needed on the within‐person fluctuation of attachment states. In this ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study, we examined (a) the associations between the baseline, variability, and inertia of each state attachment dimension (security, avoidance, and anxiety) and (b) how trait attachment (anxiety and avoidance) predicts these dynamic features.MethodIn two adult samples (Ns = 122 and 127), trait attachment dimensions were first assessed using Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised. Then, attachment states were assessed seven or ten times a day over 1 week (4629 and 5322 successful EMA observations).ResultsFor state security, individuals with high baseline exhibited lower variability. In contrast, for state avoidance, those with high baseline showed higher variability. Both trait attachment anxiety and avoidance predicted lower baseline and higher variability of state security. Moreover, both trait dimensions predicted higher baselines of the corresponding states.ConclusionsOur findings provide insights into the real‐time regulatory dynamics of the attachment system and their interconnection with trait attachment, underscoring the importance of baseline and variability in understanding how attachment manifests in everyday life.
目标依恋研究历来侧重于特质,从而加深我们对与依恋相关的个体差异的理解。然而,为了描绘依恋系统的动态特性,我们需要对依恋状态的人际波动进行更多的研究。在这项生态瞬间评估(EMA)研究中,我们考察了(a)每个依恋状态维度(安全感、回避和焦虑)的基线、变异性和惯性之间的关联,以及(b)特质依恋(焦虑和回避)如何预测这些动态特征。然后,对依恋状态进行为期一周、每天七次或十次的评估(4629 次和 5322 次成功的 EMA 观察)。相反,对于回避状态,基线高的人表现出更高的可变性。特质依恋焦虑和回避都能预测较低的状态安全感基线和较高的变异性。我们的研究结果为依恋系统的实时调节动态及其与特质依恋的相互联系提供了见解,强调了基线和变异性在理解依恋如何在日常生活中表现出来方面的重要性。
{"title":"Everyday State Attachment: Dynamic Features and Role of Trait Attachment","authors":"Jaakko Tammilehto, Aleksandra Kaurin, Guy Bosmans, Peter Kuppens, Marjo Flykt, Mervi Vänskä, Kirsi Peltonen, Jallu Lindblom","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12975","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12975","url":null,"abstract":"ObjectiveAttachment research has traditionally focused on traits, enhancing our understanding of attachment‐related individual differences. However, to chart the dynamic properties of the attachment system, more research is needed on the within‐person fluctuation of attachment states. In this ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study, we examined (a) the associations between the baseline, variability, and inertia of each state attachment dimension (security, avoidance, and anxiety) and (b) how trait attachment (anxiety and avoidance) predicts these dynamic features.MethodIn two adult samples (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic>s = 122 and 127), trait attachment dimensions were first assessed using Experiences in Close Relationships–Revised. Then, attachment states were assessed seven or ten times a day over 1 week (4629 and 5322 successful EMA observations).ResultsFor state security, individuals with high baseline exhibited lower variability. In contrast, for state avoidance, those with high baseline showed higher variability. Both trait attachment anxiety and avoidance predicted lower baseline and higher variability of state security. Moreover, both trait dimensions predicted higher baselines of the corresponding states.ConclusionsOur findings provide insights into the real‐time regulatory dynamics of the attachment system and their interconnection with trait attachment, underscoring the importance of baseline and variability in understanding how attachment manifests in everyday life.","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"11 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142448251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dispositional Traits, Characteristic Adaptations, and Narrative Identity Reconstructions in Individuals With Depersonalization and Derealization. 人格解体和去人格化患者的性格特征、特征适应和叙事身份重构。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12976
Emanuele Fino,Thalia Jemmett-Skinner,Richard Evans-Miller,Joe Perkins,Mohammed Malik,Martin Robinson,Gwendalyn Webb
INTRODUCTIONDepersonalization and derealization disorder (DPDR) is a debilitating condition. To date, little was known about the role of personality structure and of perceived social support and loneliness in DPDR.METHODSThree studies investigated, respectively: (i) broadband personality traits (five-factor model), maladaptive trait domains (PID-5), and perceived support and loneliness in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 160) versus a general population sample (N = 303), using network modeling; (ii) structure and interconnectivity of personality, perceived support and loneliness, and DPDR traits (frequency/duration) in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 160); (iii) characteristic adaptations and narrative identities in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 19), using thematic analysis.RESULTSStudy 1 found between-samples differences across several traits, especially psychoticism and negative affect. Differences in networks' global centrality, but not structures or edges, were also found. The graphical model in Study 2 showed a community of dissociative tendencies including DPDR traits and psychoticism. Study 3 highlighted the development of DPDR as a key life transition for those experiencing it, with narratives focusing on feelings of poor agency, isolation, and a disrupted sense of self.CONCLUSIONSIndividual differences in personality characterize DPDR, especially in psychoticism. Implications for theory and research are discussed.
简介:人格解体和去理想化障碍(DPDR)是一种使人衰弱的疾病。迄今为止,人们对人格结构以及感知到的社会支持和孤独感在 DPDR 中的作用知之甚少。方法三项研究分别调查了(i) 采用网络建模法,调查了自述有 DPDR 的个体(N = 160)与普通人群样本(N = 303)的宽带人格特质(五因素模型)、适应不良特质域(PID-5)以及感知到的支持和孤独感;(ii) 自我报告的 DPDR 患者(160 人)的人格、感知到的支持和孤独感以及 DPDR 特征(频率/持续时间)的结构和相互关联性;(iii) 采用主题分析法,研究自我报告的 DPDR 患者(19 人)的特征适应性和叙事身份。结果 研究 1 发现了多个特质的样本间差异,尤其是精神病性和负性情感。研究还发现了网络全局中心性的差异,但没有发现结构或边缘的差异。研究 2 的图形模型显示了一个解离倾向群体,包括 DPDR 特质和精神病性。研究 3 强调了 DPDR 的发展是那些经历过 DPDR 的人的一个关键的人生转变,其叙述主要集中在代理能力差、孤立和自我意识混乱的感觉上。讨论了其对理论和研究的影响。
{"title":"Dispositional Traits, Characteristic Adaptations, and Narrative Identity Reconstructions in Individuals With Depersonalization and Derealization.","authors":"Emanuele Fino,Thalia Jemmett-Skinner,Richard Evans-Miller,Joe Perkins,Mohammed Malik,Martin Robinson,Gwendalyn Webb","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12976","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12976","url":null,"abstract":"INTRODUCTIONDepersonalization and derealization disorder (DPDR) is a debilitating condition. To date, little was known about the role of personality structure and of perceived social support and loneliness in DPDR.METHODSThree studies investigated, respectively: (i) broadband personality traits (five-factor model), maladaptive trait domains (PID-5), and perceived support and loneliness in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 160) versus a general population sample (N = 303), using network modeling; (ii) structure and interconnectivity of personality, perceived support and loneliness, and DPDR traits (frequency/duration) in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 160); (iii) characteristic adaptations and narrative identities in individuals with self-reported DPDR (N = 19), using thematic analysis.RESULTSStudy 1 found between-samples differences across several traits, especially psychoticism and negative affect. Differences in networks' global centrality, but not structures or edges, were also found. The graphical model in Study 2 showed a community of dissociative tendencies including DPDR traits and psychoticism. Study 3 highlighted the development of DPDR as a key life transition for those experiencing it, with narratives focusing on feelings of poor agency, isolation, and a disrupted sense of self.CONCLUSIONSIndividual differences in personality characterize DPDR, especially in psychoticism. Implications for theory and research are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142447993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Farewell to the Narcissism Epidemic? A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Global NPI Scores (1982-2023). 告别自恋流行病?全球 NPI 分数的跨时空 Meta 分析(1982-2023 年)》。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12982
Sandra Oberleiter,Paul Stickel,Jakob Pietschnig
OBJECTIVESeveral recent accounts have failed to replicate the so-called Narcissism Epidemic, suggesting potential influences of the Global Financial Crisis (GFC) in 2008 as a reason for narcissism trend reversals. Here, we provide evidence for narcissism test score changes from 1982 to 2023.METHODSWe investigated self-report data on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) from 1105 studies (k = 1621, N = 546,225) using precision-weighted cross-temporal meta-analysis.RESULTSData collection years were meaningfully negatively associated with narcissism scores in virtually all analyses (bs: -0.409 to -0.008; partial eta square's: < 0.001 to 0.118; ps: < 0.001 to 0.174), thus indicating cross-temporally decreasing narcissism self-report scores. Examination of regression segments pre- and post-dating the GFC and segmented line regressions indicated mostly stable narcissism scores during the 1980s and 1990s that subsequently showed negative slopes with somewhat differing decreases onsets according to analytical subsets.CONCLUSIONSHere, we provide evidence for negative cross-temporal changes in narcissism from 1982 to 2023 globally, thus contrasting the idea of a Narcissism Epidemic having taken place at any point during the past four decades. Changes appear to generalize across different regions and participant sex, although mean scores were differentiated, yielding higher narcissism values for North American and younger samples.
目的最近的一些研究未能证实所谓的 "自恋流行病"(Narcissism Epidemic),这表明 2008 年的全球金融危机(GFC)可能是自恋趋势逆转的原因之一。在此,我们提供了从 1982 年到 2023 年自恋测试得分变化的证据。方法:我们使用精确加权的跨时空荟萃分析方法,调查了来自 1105 项研究(k = 1621,N = 546,225 人)的自恋人格量表(NPI)自我报告数据。结果:在几乎所有分析中,数据收集年份与自恋得分均呈有意义的负相关(BS:-0.409 到 -0.008;部分 eta 平方:<0.001至0.118;PS:<0.001至0.174),从而表明自恋自我报告分数呈跨时空下降趋势。对全球金融危机前后的回归片段和分段线性回归的研究表明,20 世纪 80 年代和 90 年代的自恋分数基本保持稳定,随后出现了负斜率,根据分析子集的不同,下降的起始点也略有不同。尽管平均得分存在差异,北美和年轻样本的自恋值更高,但不同地区和不同性别的参与者的变化似乎具有普遍性。
{"title":"A Farewell to the Narcissism Epidemic? A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Global NPI Scores (1982-2023).","authors":"Sandra Oberleiter,Paul Stickel,Jakob Pietschnig","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12982","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12982","url":null,"abstract":"OBJECTIVESeveral recent accounts have failed to replicate the so-called Narcissism Epidemic, suggesting potential influences of the Global Financial Crisis (GFC) in 2008 as a reason for narcissism trend reversals. Here, we provide evidence for narcissism test score changes from 1982 to 2023.METHODSWe investigated self-report data on the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) from 1105 studies (k = 1621, N = 546,225) using precision-weighted cross-temporal meta-analysis.RESULTSData collection years were meaningfully negatively associated with narcissism scores in virtually all analyses (bs: -0.409 to -0.008; partial eta square's: < 0.001 to 0.118; ps: < 0.001 to 0.174), thus indicating cross-temporally decreasing narcissism self-report scores. Examination of regression segments pre- and post-dating the GFC and segmented line regressions indicated mostly stable narcissism scores during the 1980s and 1990s that subsequently showed negative slopes with somewhat differing decreases onsets according to analytical subsets.CONCLUSIONSHere, we provide evidence for negative cross-temporal changes in narcissism from 1982 to 2023 globally, thus contrasting the idea of a Narcissism Epidemic having taken place at any point during the past four decades. Changes appear to generalize across different regions and participant sex, although mean scores were differentiated, yielding higher narcissism values for North American and younger samples.","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"70 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142436378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Fundamental Difference in the Nature of Personal Values and Personality Traits Revealed Through Different Patterns of Stability Across Their Distributions. 个人价值观和人格特质在性质上的根本差异通过其分布的不同稳定模式得以揭示。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-04 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12979
Joshua Lake, Anat Bardi, Joanne Sneddon, Julie A Lee

Objective: Personal values and personality traits are both important aspects of personality, but much is still unknown about the fundamental differences between the constructs, including how their patterns of temporal stability compare. This paper investigated patterns of intra-individual stability in both values and traits.

Method: Quantile correlations were estimated between each of the 20 refined personal values and the same values 2 years later in a large longitudinal sample of Australian adults (N = 2875). The same was done for each of the 15 Five-Factor Model trait facets in a subsample of these participants (n = 2424).

Results: It was observed that more important values tended to remain more stable over time, while traits retained a similar stability regardless of trait strength, and frequently showed small decreases in stability at extreme levels.

Conclusions: Findings indicate that highly prioritized values may be a more central aspect of the self, and a more reliable element for predicting future outcomes, than less highly prioritized values, but in contrast, traits do not function in a way that is dependent on trait strength.

目的:个人价值观和人格特质都是人格的重要方面,但人们对这两个概念之间的基本差异,包括它们在时间上的稳定性如何比较,仍有很多不了解之处。本文研究了价值观和特质的个体内部稳定性模式:方法:在一个大型澳大利亚成年人纵向样本(N = 2875)中,分别估算了 20 种经过提炼的个人价值观与 2 年后相同价值观之间的量纲相关性。在这些参与者的一个子样本(n = 2424)中,对 15 个五因素模型特质面中的每一个进行了同样的估算:结果:观察发现,随着时间的推移,更重要的价值观往往会保持更稳定的状态,而特质则无论特质强度如何,都会保持类似的稳定性,在极端水平上,稳定性经常会出现小幅下降:研究结果表明,与优先级较低的价值观相比,优先级较高的价值观可能是自我的一个更核心的方面,也是预测未来结果的一个更可靠的因素。
{"title":"A Fundamental Difference in the Nature of Personal Values and Personality Traits Revealed Through Different Patterns of Stability Across Their Distributions.","authors":"Joshua Lake, Anat Bardi, Joanne Sneddon, Julie A Lee","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12979","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12979","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Personal values and personality traits are both important aspects of personality, but much is still unknown about the fundamental differences between the constructs, including how their patterns of temporal stability compare. This paper investigated patterns of intra-individual stability in both values and traits.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Quantile correlations were estimated between each of the 20 refined personal values and the same values 2 years later in a large longitudinal sample of Australian adults (N = 2875). The same was done for each of the 15 Five-Factor Model trait facets in a subsample of these participants (n = 2424).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>It was observed that more important values tended to remain more stable over time, while traits retained a similar stability regardless of trait strength, and frequently showed small decreases in stability at extreme levels.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings indicate that highly prioritized values may be a more central aspect of the self, and a more reliable element for predicting future outcomes, than less highly prioritized values, but in contrast, traits do not function in a way that is dependent on trait strength.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142373270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "Going It Alone: Examining Interpersonal Sensitivity and Hostility as Mediators of the Link Between Perfectionism and Social Disconnection". 独自行动:研究人际关系敏感性和敌意作为完美主义与社会隔离之间联系的中介 "一文的更正。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12977

Visvalingam, S., N. R. Magson, A. R. Newins, and M. Norberg. 2023. "Going It Alone: Examining Interpersonal Sensitivity and Hostility as Mediators of the Link Between Perfectionism and Social Disconnection." Journal of Personality 92, no. 4: 1024-1036. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12868. The authors have identified the following errors in data preparation: Data were not linked for one participant who used different ID codes across Time 1 and 3. Data for Time 3 were not excluded for one participant who failed the Time 1 attention check. The UCLA Loneliness scale was erroneously coded 0-3 instead of 1-4 and one item was repeated. These errors were corrected, and the data were reanalyzed leading to minor changes in some of the previously reported values. Four additional outliers were also identified in the corrected dataset. Importantly, the original findings and conclusions did not change. The corrected tables, models, and updated article text can be found here: https://osf.io/gza5y/?view_only=3c2fa9efced14c32aca7a9f4f5f14ab9. We apologize for these errors.

Visvalingam, S., N. R. Magson, A. R. Newins, and M. Norberg.2023."Going It Alone:考察人际敏感性和敌意作为完美主义与社会脱节之间联系的中介"。Journal of Personality 92, no.4: 1024-1036。https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12868。作者在数据准备过程中发现以下错误:一位参与者在时间 1 和时间 3 中使用了不同的 ID 码,其数据未进行链接。未排除一名未能通过时间 1 注意力检查的受试者的时间 3 数据。加州大学洛杉矶分校孤独感量表被错误地编码为 0-3,而不是 1-4,并且有一个项目被重复。这些错误已得到纠正,并对数据进行了重新分析,导致之前报告的一些数值发生了细微变化。更正后的数据集中还发现了另外四个异常值。重要的是,最初的研究结果和结论并没有改变。更正后的表格、模型和更新后的文章内容可在这里找到:https://osf.io/gza5y/?view_only=3c2fa9efced14c32aca7a9f4f5f14ab9。我们对这些错误表示歉意。
{"title":"Correction to \"Going It Alone: Examining Interpersonal Sensitivity and Hostility as Mediators of the Link Between Perfectionism and Social Disconnection\".","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12977","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12977","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visvalingam, S., N. R. Magson, A. R. Newins, and M. Norberg. 2023. \"Going It Alone: Examining Interpersonal Sensitivity and Hostility as Mediators of the Link Between Perfectionism and Social Disconnection.\" Journal of Personality 92, no. 4: 1024-1036. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12868. The authors have identified the following errors in data preparation: Data were not linked for one participant who used different ID codes across Time 1 and 3. Data for Time 3 were not excluded for one participant who failed the Time 1 attention check. The UCLA Loneliness scale was erroneously coded 0-3 instead of 1-4 and one item was repeated. These errors were corrected, and the data were reanalyzed leading to minor changes in some of the previously reported values. Four additional outliers were also identified in the corrected dataset. Importantly, the original findings and conclusions did not change. The corrected tables, models, and updated article text can be found here: https://osf.io/gza5y/?view_only=3c2fa9efced14c32aca7a9f4f5f14ab9. We apologize for these errors.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interpersonal Perception of Adult Playfulness at Zero-Acquaintance: A Conceptual Replication Study of Self-Other Agreement and Consensus, and an Extension to Two Accuracy Criteria. 人际关系中对成人零距离玩耍的感知:对自我与他人一致意见和共识的概念复制研究,以及对两种准确性标准的扩展。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12978
Kay Brauer, René T Proyer

Objective: We replicated and extended previous research examining the accuracy of judgments of four facets of adult playfulness (Other-directed, Lighthearted, Intellectual, and Whimsical; OLIW) at zero-acquaintance.

Method: We conducted a conceptual replication study. One hundred sixty targets provided self-ratings for the OLIW facets, textual self-descriptions (≤ five sentences), daily self-ratings of playfulness for 14 consecutive days, and ratings by knowledgeable others. Six unacquainted judges provided rated targets' playfulness based on their self-descriptions. We replicated findings on trait-wise self-other agreement (SOA) and consensus and extended prior research by testing SOA for profiles of the four facets and two distinct accuracy criteria (i.e., targets' diary data and aggregates of targets' self-reports and those from knowledgeable others).

Results: All interpersonal perception indicators showed that facets of playfulness can be perceived above chance (SOA ≥ 0.26; consensus ≥ 0.29, accuracies ≥ 0.16). SOA extends from single facets to profiles, also when controlling for stereotype effects.

Conclusions: Playfulness can be accurately observed from minimal textual information at zero acquaintance. Our study highlights the robustness of findings on the interpersonal perception of playfulness across samples and methods, and degrees of acquaintanceship. We discuss implications for playfulness in social relationships.

研究目的我们复制并扩展了之前的研究,考察了在零距离接触时对成人游戏性的四个方面(他人指导型、轻松愉快型、智力型和异想天开型;OLIW)判断的准确性:我们进行了一项概念复制研究。160名目标对象提供了OLIW方面的自我评分、文本自我描述(≤五句)、连续14天的游戏性每日自我评分以及了解情况的其他人的评分。六位不相识的评委根据目标人物的自我描述对其游戏性进行评分。我们重复了特质上的自我-他人一致(SOA)和共识的研究结果,并通过测试四个方面的SOA和两个不同的准确性标准(即目标的日记数据和目标的自我报告与其他知情者的报告的总和)扩展了之前的研究:结果:所有人际感知指标都表明,游戏性的各个侧面都能被感知,且高于偶然性(SOA≥0.26;共识度≥0.29,准确度≥0.16)。在控制刻板印象效应的情况下,SOA 也能从单一侧面扩展到侧面:结论:在零相识度的情况下,可以从最少的文本信息中准确地观察到玩乐性。我们的研究凸显了不同样本、不同方法和不同熟识程度的人际间对游戏性感知结果的稳健性。我们讨论了游戏性在社会关系中的意义。
{"title":"Interpersonal Perception of Adult Playfulness at Zero-Acquaintance: A Conceptual Replication Study of Self-Other Agreement and Consensus, and an Extension to Two Accuracy Criteria.","authors":"Kay Brauer, René T Proyer","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12978","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12978","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We replicated and extended previous research examining the accuracy of judgments of four facets of adult playfulness (Other-directed, Lighthearted, Intellectual, and Whimsical; OLIW) at zero-acquaintance.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We conducted a conceptual replication study. One hundred sixty targets provided self-ratings for the OLIW facets, textual self-descriptions (≤ five sentences), daily self-ratings of playfulness for 14 consecutive days, and ratings by knowledgeable others. Six unacquainted judges provided rated targets' playfulness based on their self-descriptions. We replicated findings on trait-wise self-other agreement (SOA) and consensus and extended prior research by testing SOA for profiles of the four facets and two distinct accuracy criteria (i.e., targets' diary data and aggregates of targets' self-reports and those from knowledgeable others).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>All interpersonal perception indicators showed that facets of playfulness can be perceived above chance (SOA ≥ 0.26; consensus ≥ 0.29, accuracies ≥ 0.16). SOA extends from single facets to profiles, also when controlling for stereotype effects.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Playfulness can be accurately observed from minimal textual information at zero acquaintance. Our study highlights the robustness of findings on the interpersonal perception of playfulness across samples and methods, and degrees of acquaintanceship. We discuss implications for playfulness in social relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1