首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
The effects of filler similarity and lineup size on eyewitness identification. 填充物相似性和阵容大小对目击者识别的影响。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001342
Kyros J Shen, Jiaqi Huang, Allan L Lam, John T Wixted

A photo lineup, which is a cross between an old/new and a forced-choice recognition memory test, consists of one suspect, whose face was either seen before or not, and several physically similar fillers. First, the participant/witness must decide whether the person who was previously seen is present (old/new) and then, if present, choose the previously seen target (forced choice). Competing signal-detection models of eyewitness identification performance make different predictions about how certain variables will affect a witness's ability to discriminate previously seen (guilty) suspects from new (innocent) suspects. One key variable is the similarity of the fillers to the suspect in the lineup, and another key variable is the size of the lineup (i.e., the number of fillers). Previous research investigating the role of filler similarity has supported one model, known as the Ensemble model, whereas previous research investigating the role of lineup size has supported a competing model, known as the Independent Observations model. We simultaneously manipulated these two variables (filler similarity and lineup size) and found a pattern that is not predicted by either model. When the fillers were highly similar to the suspect, increasing lineup size reduced discriminability, but when the fillers were dissimilar to the suspect, increasing lineup size enhanced discriminability. The results suggest that each additional filler adds noise to the decision-making process and that this noise factor is minimized by maximizing filler dissimilarity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

列队照相是一种介于 "新旧 "和 "强迫选择 "之间的辨认记忆测试,由一名嫌疑人(其面孔要么以前见过,要么没见过)和几名体貌相似的填充物组成。首先,参与者/证人必须判断之前见过的人是否在场(旧/新),如果在场,则选择之前见过的目标(强迫选择)。对于某些变量会如何影响证人辨别以前见过的(有罪的)嫌疑人和新的(无罪的)嫌疑人的能力,目击者辨认能力的信号检测模型之间存在竞争,并做出了不同的预测。其中一个关键变量是列队中填充物与嫌疑人的相似度,另一个关键变量是列队的大小(即填充物的数量)。以往调查填充物相似性作用的研究支持一个模型,即 "集合模型",而以往调查列队人数作用的研究支持一个竞争模型,即 "独立观察模型"。我们同时操纵了这两个变量(填充物相似度和阵容大小),结果发现了一个两个模型都无法预测的模式。当填充物与嫌疑人高度相似时,增加列队人数会降低辨别能力,但当填充物与嫌疑人不相似时,增加列队人数会提高辨别能力。结果表明,每增加一个填充者都会给决策过程增加噪音,而通过最大限度地提高填充者的不相似性,可以最大限度地降低噪音因素。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effects of filler similarity and lineup size on eyewitness identification.","authors":"Kyros J Shen, Jiaqi Huang, Allan L Lam, John T Wixted","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001342","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001342","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A photo lineup, which is a cross between an old/new and a forced-choice recognition memory test, consists of one suspect, whose face was either seen before or not, and several physically similar fillers. First, the participant/witness must decide whether the person who was previously seen is present (old/new) and then, if present, choose the previously seen target (forced choice). Competing signal-detection models of eyewitness identification performance make different predictions about how certain variables will affect a witness's ability to discriminate previously seen (guilty) suspects from new (innocent) suspects. One key variable is the similarity of the fillers to the suspect in the lineup, and another key variable is the size of the lineup (i.e., the number of fillers). Previous research investigating the role of filler similarity has supported one model, known as the Ensemble model, whereas previous research investigating the role of lineup size has supported a competing model, known as the Independent Observations model. We simultaneously manipulated these two variables (filler similarity and lineup size) and found a pattern that is not predicted by either model. When the fillers were highly similar to the suspect, increasing lineup size reduced discriminability, but when the fillers were dissimilar to the suspect, increasing lineup size enhanced discriminability. The results suggest that each additional filler adds noise to the decision-making process and that this noise factor is minimized by maximizing filler dissimilarity. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140871200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conceptual information of meaningful objects is stored incidentally. 有意义对象的概念信息是偶然存储的。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001339
Yong Hoon Chung, Joyce Tam, Brad Wyble, Viola S Störmer

Prior research has shown that visual working memory capacity is enhanced for meaningful stimuli (i.e., real-world objects) compared to abstract shapes (i.e., colored circles). Here, we hypothesized that the shape of meaningful objects would be better remembered incidentally than the shape of nonmeaningful objects in a color memory task where the shape of the objects is task-irrelevant. We used a surprise-trial paradigm in which participants performed a color memory task for several trials before being probed with a surprise trial that asked them about the shape of the last object they saw. Across three experiments, we found a memory advantage for recognizable shapes relative to scrambled versions of these shapes (Experiment 1) that was robust across different encoding times (Experiment 2), and the addition of a verbal suppression task (Experiment 3). Interestingly, this advantage disappeared when all objects were from the same category (Experiment 4), suggesting that people are incidentally encoding broad conceptual information about object identities, but not visual details. Finally, when we asked about the location of objects in a surprise trial, we did not observe any difference between the two stimulus types (Experiment 5). Overall, these results show that conceptual information about the categories of meaningful objects is incidentally encoded into working memory even when task-irrelevant. This privilege for meaningful information does not exhibit a trade-off with location memory, suggesting that meaningful features influence representations of visual working memory in higher-level visual regions without altering the use of spatial reference frames at the lower level. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

先前的研究表明,与抽象的形状(即彩色圆圈)相比,有意义的刺激物(即现实世界中的物体)会增强视觉工作记忆能力。在此,我们假设在颜色记忆任务中,如果物体的形状与任务无关,那么有意义物体的形状会比无意义物体的形状更容易被记住。我们采用了一个惊喜试验范式,让被试在完成几次颜色记忆任务后,再进行一次惊喜试验,询问他们最后看到的物体的形状。在三个实验中,我们发现相对于这些形状的乱码版本,可识别形状具有记忆优势(实验 1),这种优势在不同的编码时间(实验 2)和附加的言语抑制任务(实验 3)中都很稳固。有趣的是,当所有物体都属于同一类别时,这种优势就消失了(实验 4),这表明人们是在偶然编码有关物体特征的广泛概念信息,而不是视觉细节。最后,当我们在惊喜试验中询问物体的位置时,我们没有观察到两种刺激类型之间有任何差异(实验 5)。总之,这些结果表明,即使在与任务无关的情况下,有关有意义物体类别的概念信息也会被偶然编码到工作记忆中。这种对有意义信息的特权并没有表现出与位置记忆的权衡,这表明有意义的特征会影响较高层次视觉区域的视觉工作记忆表征,而不会改变较低层次空间参照框架的使用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Conceptual information of meaningful objects is stored incidentally.","authors":"Yong Hoon Chung, Joyce Tam, Brad Wyble, Viola S Störmer","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001339","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001339","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has shown that visual working memory capacity is enhanced for meaningful stimuli (i.e., real-world objects) compared to abstract shapes (i.e., colored circles). Here, we hypothesized that the shape of meaningful objects would be better remembered incidentally than the shape of nonmeaningful objects in a color memory task where the shape of the objects is task-irrelevant. We used a surprise-trial paradigm in which participants performed a color memory task for several trials before being probed with a surprise trial that asked them about the shape of the last object they saw. Across three experiments, we found a memory advantage for recognizable shapes relative to scrambled versions of these shapes (Experiment 1) that was robust across different encoding times (Experiment 2), and the addition of a verbal suppression task (Experiment 3). Interestingly, this advantage disappeared when all objects were from the same category (Experiment 4), suggesting that people are incidentally encoding broad conceptual information about object identities, but not visual details. Finally, when we asked about the location of objects in a surprise trial, we did not observe any difference between the two stimulus types (Experiment 5). Overall, these results show that conceptual information about the categories of meaningful objects is incidentally encoded into working memory even when task-irrelevant. This privilege for meaningful information does not exhibit a trade-off with location memory, suggesting that meaningful features influence representations of visual working memory in higher-level visual regions without altering the use of spatial reference frames at the lower level. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140853349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direction-specific reading experience shapes perceptual span. 特定方向的阅读经验塑造了知觉跨度。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001340
Ming Yan, Reinhold Kliegl, Jinger Pan

Perceptual span in reading, the spatial extent for effective information extraction during a single fixation, provides a critical foundation to all studies for sentence reading. However, it is not understood fully how the perceptual span is influenced by direction-specific reading experience. Traditional Chinese sentences can be written horizontally from left to right or vertically downward, offering the best opportunity to explore readers' perceptual span in different text directions, free of possible confounding with language proficiency and cross-participant differences. Using a within-item and within-subject design, eye movements of traditional Chinese readers were recorded during their reading of horizontally and vertically presented sentences. Additionally, regardless of text direction, a gaze-contingent moving-window technique was adopted to restrict visible texts within a virtual window that moved in synchrony with the reader's eye gaze, while characters outside the window were masked. Among several critical results, most importantly, asymptotic reading performance was observed in a smaller window condition for vertical reading than for horizontal reading, suggesting an overall smaller perceptual span in the former case. In addition, the size of the vertical perceptual span increased as a function of the readers' familiarity with vertical text. We conclude that factors beyond orthographic complexity and readers' language proficiency can influence the way in which humans read. Readers' direction-specific perceptual experiences can influence their perceptual span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

阅读中的感知跨度是指在单次定影过程中有效提取信息的空间范围,它为所有句子阅读研究提供了重要基础。然而,感知跨度如何受到特定方向阅读经验的影响,目前尚不完全清楚。繁体中文句子可以从左到右横向书写,也可以从上到下纵向书写,这为研究读者在不同文字方向上的感知跨度提供了最佳机会,同时也避免了语言能力和跨参与者差异可能造成的混淆。通过项目内和被试内设计,我们记录了繁体中文读者在阅读水平和垂直呈现的句子时的眼球运动。此外,无论文字的方向如何,我们都采用了一种凝视视变移动窗口技术,将可见文字限制在一个虚拟窗口内,该窗口会随着读者眼睛的凝视同步移动,而窗口外的字符则被遮挡。在几项关键结果中,最重要的是,与水平阅读相比,垂直阅读的窗口条件更小,这表明前者的总体感知跨度更小。此外,垂直感知跨度的大小随读者对垂直文本的熟悉程度而增加。我们的结论是,除了正字法的复杂性和读者的语言能力之外,其他因素也会影响人类的阅读方式。读者对特定方向的感知经验会影响他们的感知跨度。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Direction-specific reading experience shapes perceptual span.","authors":"Ming Yan, Reinhold Kliegl, Jinger Pan","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001340","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001340","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceptual span in reading, the spatial extent for effective information extraction during a single fixation, provides a critical foundation to all studies for sentence reading. However, it is not understood fully how the perceptual span is influenced by direction-specific reading experience. Traditional Chinese sentences can be written horizontally from left to right or vertically downward, offering the best opportunity to explore readers' perceptual span in different text directions, free of possible confounding with language proficiency and cross-participant differences. Using a within-item and within-subject design, eye movements of traditional Chinese readers were recorded during their reading of horizontally and vertically presented sentences. Additionally, regardless of text direction, a gaze-contingent moving-window technique was adopted to restrict visible texts within a virtual window that moved in synchrony with the reader's eye gaze, while characters outside the window were masked. Among several critical results, most importantly, asymptotic reading performance was observed in a smaller window condition for vertical reading than for horizontal reading, suggesting an overall smaller perceptual span in the former case. In addition, the size of the vertical perceptual span increased as a function of the readers' familiarity with vertical text. We conclude that factors beyond orthographic complexity and readers' language proficiency can influence the way in which humans read. Readers' direction-specific perceptual experiences can influence their perceptual span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140856790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memory modeling of counterfactual generation. 反事实生成的记忆建模。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001335
Feiyi Wang, Ada Aka, Lisheng He, Sudeep Bhatia

We use a computational model of memory search to study how people generate counterfactual outcomes in response to an established target outcome. Hierarchical Bayesian model fitting to data from six experiments reveals that counterfactual outcomes that are perceived as more desirable and more likely to occur are also more likely to come to mind and are generated earlier than other outcomes. Additionally, core memory mechanisms such as semantic clustering and word frequency biases have a strong influence on retrieval dynamics in counterfactual thinking. Finally, we find that the set of counterfactuals that come to mind can be manipulated by modifying the total number of counterfactuals that participants are prompted to generate, and our model can predict these effects. Overall, our findings demonstrate how computational memory search models can be integrated with current theories of counterfactual thinking to provide novel insights into the process of generating counterfactual thoughts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们利用记忆搜索的计算模型来研究人们如何针对既定的目标结果产生反事实结果。对来自六个实验的数据进行分层贝叶斯模型拟合后发现,与其他结果相比,人们认为更理想、更有可能发生的反事实结果也更有可能出现在脑海中,并且更早产生。此外,语义聚类和词频偏差等核心记忆机制对反事实思维的检索动态有很大影响。最后,我们发现,可以通过改变参与者被提示生成的反事实的总数来操纵所想到的反事实的集合,而我们的模型可以预测这些效果。总之,我们的研究结果表明,计算记忆搜索模型可以与当前的反事实思维理论相结合,为反事实思维的产生过程提供新的见解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Memory modeling of counterfactual generation.","authors":"Feiyi Wang, Ada Aka, Lisheng He, Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001335","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001335","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We use a computational model of memory search to study how people generate counterfactual outcomes in response to an established target outcome. Hierarchical Bayesian model fitting to data from six experiments reveals that counterfactual outcomes that are perceived as more desirable and more likely to occur are also more likely to come to mind and are generated earlier than other outcomes. Additionally, core memory mechanisms such as semantic clustering and word frequency biases have a strong influence on retrieval dynamics in counterfactual thinking. Finally, we find that the set of counterfactuals that come to mind can be manipulated by modifying the total number of counterfactuals that participants are prompted to generate, and our model can predict these effects. Overall, our findings demonstrate how computational memory search models can be integrated with current theories of counterfactual thinking to provide novel insights into the process of generating counterfactual thoughts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140865703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Saccadic targeting in the Landolt-C task: Implications for Chinese reading. Landolt-C任务中的游标瞄准:对中文阅读的启示。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001343
Xinyi Xia, Qin Liu, Erik D Reichle, Yanping Liu

Participants in an eye-movement experiment performed a modified version of the Landolt-C paradigm (Williams & Pollatsek, 2007) to determine if there are preferred viewing locations when they searched for target squares embedded in linear arrays of spatially contiguous clusters of squares (i.e., sequences of one to four squares having missing segments of variable size and orientation). The results of this experiment indicate that, although the peaks of the single- and first-of-multiple-fixation landing-site distributions were respectively located near the centers and beginnings of the clusters, thereby replicating previous patterns that have been interpreted as evidence for the default saccadic-targeting hypothesis, the same dissociation was evident on nonclusters (i.e., arbitrarily defined regions of analysis). Furthermore, properties of the clusters (e.g., character number and gap size) influenced fixation durations and forward saccade length, suggesting that ongoing stimulus processing affects decisions about when and where (i.e., how far) to move the eyes. Finally, results of simulations using simple oculomotor-based, default-targeting, and dynamic-adjustment models indicated that the latter performed better than the other two, suggesting that the dynamic-adjustment strategy likely reflects the basic perceptual and motor constraints shared by a variety of visual tasks, rather than being specific to Chinese reading. The theoretical implications of these results for existing and future accounts of eye-movement control are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在一项眼动实验中,受试者进行了改进版的 Landolt-C 范式(Williams & Pollatsek, 2007),以确定当他们搜索嵌入空间连续方格簇线性阵列中的目标方格(即由一到四个方格组成的序列,具有大小和方向可变的缺失片段)时,是否存在偏好的观察位置。实验结果表明,虽然单次定点和多次定点着陆点分布的峰值分别位于方格簇的中心和起点附近,从而复制了之前被解释为默认囊回定点假说证据的模式,但同样的分离在非方格簇(即任意定义的分析区域)上也很明显。此外,簇的属性(如字符数和间隙大小)也会影响固定持续时间和前向囊状移动长度,这表明正在进行的刺激处理会影响关于何时何地(即移动多远)移动眼睛的决策。最后,使用简单的基于眼球运动的模型、默认目标模型和动态调整模型进行模拟的结果表明,后者的表现优于其他两种模型,这表明动态调整策略很可能反映了各种视觉任务所共有的基本知觉和运动限制,而不是中文阅读所特有的。本文讨论了这些结果对现有和未来眼动控制理论的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Saccadic targeting in the Landolt-C task: Implications for Chinese reading.","authors":"Xinyi Xia, Qin Liu, Erik D Reichle, Yanping Liu","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001343","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001343","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Participants in an eye-movement experiment performed a modified version of the Landolt-C paradigm (Williams & Pollatsek, 2007) to determine if there are preferred viewing locations when they searched for target squares embedded in linear arrays of spatially contiguous clusters of squares (i.e., sequences of one to four squares having missing segments of variable size and orientation). The results of this experiment indicate that, although the peaks of the single- and first-of-multiple-fixation landing-site distributions were respectively located near the centers and beginnings of the clusters, thereby replicating previous patterns that have been interpreted as evidence for the default saccadic-targeting hypothesis, the same dissociation was evident on nonclusters (i.e., arbitrarily defined regions of analysis). Furthermore, properties of the clusters (e.g., character number and gap size) influenced fixation durations and forward saccade length, suggesting that ongoing stimulus processing affects decisions about when and where (i.e., how far) to move the eyes. Finally, results of simulations using simple oculomotor-based, default-targeting, and dynamic-adjustment models indicated that the latter performed better than the other two, suggesting that the dynamic-adjustment strategy likely reflects the basic perceptual and motor constraints shared by a variety of visual tasks, rather than being specific to Chinese reading. The theoretical implications of these results for existing and future accounts of eye-movement control are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Accessibility of working memory representations in the focus of attention: Heightened or reduced? 工作记忆表征在注意力集中的可及性:提高还是降低?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001223
Caro Hautekiet, Naomi Langerock, Evie Vergauwe

Many researchers agree that information residing in the focus of attention in working memory benefits from a boost in memory strength and activation, as well as heightened accessibility. However, recent studies have questioned this heightened accessibility. More specifically, these recent studies found reduced accessibility for an item in the focus of attention compared to another item in working memory, which was referred to as an "inhibition-of-return-like" effect. Our study aimed to provide a detailed examination of the accessibility of information in the focus of attention. Across a series of experiments, varying task characteristics related to the time course of the effect (Experiments 2-3) and the potential role of response inhibition (Experiments 4a-4b), we repeatedly failed to find evidence for an inhibition-of-return-like effect. Instead, we mostly found heightened accessibility for an item in the focus of attention. Given that an inhibition-of-return-like effect seems to be limited to a very specific task condition, reduced accessibility of information in the focus of attention appears to be far from a general phenomenon. Therefore, based on our findings, we propose that information in the focus of attention generally benefits from heightened accessibility, although there may be factors, most likely unrelated to the functioning of the focus of attention, that could sometimes mask this and even result in data patterns that are consistent with reduced accessibility. The theoretical implications for the focus of attention and working memory are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多研究人员一致认为,工作记忆中注意力集中的信息受益于记忆强度和激活的提高,以及可访问性的提高。然而,最近的研究对这种可访问性的提高提出了质疑。更具体地说,最近的这些研究发现,与工作记忆中的另一个项目相比,注意力焦点中的一个项目的可及性降低,这被称为“类返回抑制”效应。我们的研究旨在对关注焦点中信息的可访问性进行详细的检查。在一系列实验中,不同的任务特征与效应的时间进程有关(实验2-3)和反应抑制的潜在作用(实验4a-4b),我们一再未能找到抑制返回样效应的证据。相反,我们大多发现关注焦点中的某个项目的可访问性提高了。考虑到类似回报效应的抑制似乎仅限于一个非常特定的任务条件,注意力集中的信息可访问性降低似乎远非普遍现象。因此,根据我们的研究结果,我们提出,关注焦点中的信息通常受益于可访问性的提高,尽管可能有一些因素,很可能与关注焦点的功能无关,有时会掩盖这一点,甚至导致与可访问性降低相一致的数据模式。讨论了注意力集中和工作记忆的理论含义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Accessibility of working memory representations in the focus of attention: Heightened or reduced?","authors":"Caro Hautekiet, Naomi Langerock, Evie Vergauwe","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001223","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001223","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many researchers agree that information residing in the focus of attention in working memory benefits from a boost in memory strength and activation, as well as heightened accessibility. However, recent studies have questioned this heightened accessibility. More specifically, these recent studies found reduced accessibility for an item in the focus of attention compared to another item in working memory, which was referred to as an \"inhibition-of-return-like\" effect. Our study aimed to provide a detailed examination of the accessibility of information in the focus of attention. Across a series of experiments, varying task characteristics related to the time course of the effect (Experiments 2-3) and the potential role of response inhibition (Experiments 4a-4b), we repeatedly failed to find evidence for an inhibition-of-return-like effect. Instead, we mostly found heightened accessibility for an item in the focus of attention. Given that an inhibition-of-return-like effect seems to be limited to a very specific task condition, reduced accessibility of information in the focus of attention appears to be far from a general phenomenon. Therefore, based on our findings, we propose that information in the focus of attention generally benefits from heightened accessibility, although there may be factors, most likely unrelated to the functioning of the focus of attention, that could sometimes mask this and even result in data patterns that are consistent with reduced accessibility. The theoretical implications for the focus of attention and working memory are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41219078","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual learning changes the amplitude not the shape of the temporal window of visual processing. 知觉学习改变的是视觉加工时间窗口的振幅而非形状。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-11 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001258
Liang Lin, Xiaowei Ruan, Renjie Liu, Jinli Zhu, Wenhua Zhang, Zhong-Lin Lu, Fan Lu, Fang Hou

Perceptual learning (PL) can significantly improve human performance in perceptual tasks primarily through template reweighting. Previous studies have documented how PL changes perceptual template in stimulus feature space. We investigated how PL reweights visual information in time. With a dynamic external noise paradigm and the elaborated perceptual template model (ePTM) analysis, we found that training with an orientation identification task in the zero external noise condition reduced contrast thresholds in both zero and high external noise conditions, whereas training in the high external noise condition only reduced contrast thresholds in high external noise conditions. The ePTM analysis showed that training in both zero and high external noise changed the overall amplitude, but not the shape of the temporal window of the perceptual template to exclude external noise across time, and training in zero external noise additionally reduced additive internal noise. Our results provided additional constraints for models of PL. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

知觉学习(Perceptual Learning,PL)主要通过模板重权来显著提高人类在知觉任务中的表现。以往的研究已经记录了感知学习如何改变刺激特征空间中的感知模板。我们研究了 PL 如何在时间上对视觉信息重新加权。通过动态外部噪声范例和精心设计的感知模板模型(ePTM)分析,我们发现在零外部噪声条件下进行方位识别任务训练可降低零外部噪声和高外部噪声条件下的对比度阈值,而在高外部噪声条件下进行的训练只能降低高外部噪声条件下的对比度阈值。ePTM 分析表明,在零外部噪音和高外部噪音条件下进行的训练改变了感知模板的整体振幅,但没有改变其时间窗口的形状,从而在不同时间段排除了外部噪音。我们的研究结果为PL模型提供了额外的约束条件。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceptual learning changes the amplitude not the shape of the temporal window of visual processing.","authors":"Liang Lin, Xiaowei Ruan, Renjie Liu, Jinli Zhu, Wenhua Zhang, Zhong-Lin Lu, Fan Lu, Fang Hou","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001258","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001258","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceptual learning (PL) can significantly improve human performance in perceptual tasks primarily through template reweighting. Previous studies have documented how PL changes perceptual template in stimulus feature space. We investigated how PL reweights visual information in time. With a dynamic external noise paradigm and the elaborated perceptual template model (ePTM) analysis, we found that training with an orientation identification task in the zero external noise condition reduced contrast thresholds in both zero and high external noise conditions, whereas training in the high external noise condition only reduced contrast thresholds in high external noise conditions. The ePTM analysis showed that training in both zero and high external noise changed the overall amplitude, but not the shape of the temporal window of the perceptual template to exclude external noise across time, and training in zero external noise additionally reduced additive internal noise. Our results provided additional constraints for models of PL. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9796964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual similarity judgments do not predict the distribution of errors in working memory. 知觉相似性判断无法预测工作记忆中的错误分布。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2022-11-28 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001172
Ivan Tomić, Paul M Bays

Population coding models provide a quantitative account of visual working memory (VWM) retrieval errors with a plausible link to the response characteristics of sensory neurons. Recent work has provided an important new perspective linking population coding to variables of signal detection, including d-prime, and put forward a new hypothesis: that the distribution of recall errors on, for example, a color wheel, is a consequence of the psychological similarity between points in that stimulus space, such that the exponential-like psychophysical distance scaling function can fulfil the role of population tuning and obviate the need to fit a tuning width parameter to recall data. Using four different visual feature spaces, we measured psychophysical similarity and memory errors in the same participants. Our results revealed strong evidence for a common source of variability affecting similarity judgments and recall estimates but did not support any consistent relationship between psychophysical similarity functions and VWM errors. At the group level, the responsiveness functions obtained from the psychophysical similarity task diverged strongly from those that provided the best fit to working memory errors. At the individual level, we found convincing evidence against an association between observed and best-fitting similarity functions. Finally, our results show that the newly proposed exponential-like responsiveness function has in general no advantage over the canonical von Mises (circular normal) function assumed by previous population coding models. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

群体编码模型提供了视觉工作记忆(VWM)检索错误的定量解释,并与感觉神经元的反应特征有着可信的联系。最近的研究提供了一个重要的新视角,将群体编码与信号检测变量(包括d-prime)联系起来,并提出了一个新假设:色轮等的检索错误分布是该刺激空间中各点之间心理相似性的结果,这样类似指数的心理物理距离缩放函数就能发挥群体调谐的作用,而无需将调谐宽度参数拟合到检索数据中。我们使用四个不同的视觉特征空间,测量了同一参与者的心理物理相似性和记忆错误。我们的结果有力地证明了影响相似性判断和回忆估计的共同变异性来源,但并不支持心理物理相似性函数和 VWM 误差之间的任何一致关系。在群体水平上,从心理物理相似性任务中获得的反应性函数与工作记忆错误的最佳拟合函数有很大差异。在个体层面上,我们发现了令人信服的证据,表明观察到的相似性函数与最佳拟合相似性函数之间并不存在关联。最后,我们的研究结果表明,与以前的群体编码模型所假设的典型冯-米塞斯(圆形正态)函数相比,新提出的类似指数的反应性函数总体上没有优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceptual similarity judgments do not predict the distribution of errors in working memory.","authors":"Ivan Tomić, Paul M Bays","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001172","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001172","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Population coding models provide a quantitative account of visual working memory (VWM) retrieval errors with a plausible link to the response characteristics of sensory neurons. Recent work has provided an important new perspective linking population coding to variables of signal detection, including d-prime, and put forward a new hypothesis: that the distribution of recall errors on, for example, a color wheel, is a consequence of the psychological similarity between points in that stimulus space, such that the exponential-like psychophysical distance scaling function can fulfil the role of population tuning and obviate the need to fit a tuning width parameter to recall data. Using four different visual feature spaces, we measured psychophysical similarity and memory errors in the same participants. Our results revealed strong evidence for a common source of variability affecting similarity judgments and recall estimates but did not support any consistent relationship between psychophysical similarity functions and VWM errors. At the group level, the responsiveness functions obtained from the psychophysical similarity task diverged strongly from those that provided the best fit to working memory errors. At the individual level, we found convincing evidence against an association between observed and best-fitting similarity functions. Finally, our results show that the newly proposed exponential-like responsiveness function has in general no advantage over the canonical von Mises (circular normal) function assumed by previous population coding models. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7615806/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40708998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trust my gesture or my word: How do listeners choose the information channel during communication? 相信我的手势还是我的话:在交流过程中,听众如何选择信息渠道?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-08 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001253
Burcu Arslan, Francis Ng, Tilbe Göksun, Nazbanou Nozari

Information can be conveyed via multiple channels such as verbal and gestural (visual) channels during communication. Sometimes the information from different channels does not match (e.g., saying right while pointing to the left). How do addressees choose which information to act upon in such cases? In two experiments, we investigated this issue by having participants follow instructions on how to move objects on the screen. Experiment 1 examined whether people's choice of channel can be altered by feedback favoring either the verbal or the gestural channel. In Experiment 2, there was no feedback and participants were free to choose either channel. We also assessed participants' verbal and visuospatial working memory capacities. Results showed that, when faced with contradicting information, there is a natural bias at the group level toward relying on the verbal channel, although this bias can be temporarily altered by probabilistic feedback. Moreover, when labels were shorter and of higher frequency, participants relied more on the verbal channel. In the absence of feedback, the capacity of individuals' visual, but not verbal, working memory determined reliance on one channel versus the other. Collectively, these results show that information selection in communication is influenced by group-level biases, as well as the properties of items and characteristics of individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在交流过程中,信息可以通过多种渠道传递,如语言和手势(视觉)渠道。有时,来自不同渠道的信息并不一致(例如,一边说向右,一边却指向左边)。在这种情况下,受话人如何选择根据哪些信息采取行动呢?在两个实验中,我们通过让受试者按照指示移动屏幕上的物体来研究这个问题。实验 1 考察了人们对信息渠道的选择是否会因为偏向语言渠道或手势渠道的反馈而改变。在实验 2 中,没有任何反馈,参与者可以自由选择任一通道。我们还评估了参与者的语言和视觉空间工作记忆能力。结果表明,当面对相互矛盾的信息时,在群体水平上存在着依赖语言渠道的自然偏差,尽管这种偏差可以通过概率反馈暂时改变。此外,当标签较短且频率较高时,参与者更依赖于语言渠道。在没有反馈的情况下,个人的视觉工作记忆能力而不是语言工作记忆能力决定了对一种渠道的依赖程度。总之,这些结果表明,交流中的信息选择受到群体层面的偏差以及项目属性和个体特征的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Trust my gesture or my word: How do listeners choose the information channel during communication?","authors":"Burcu Arslan, Francis Ng, Tilbe Göksun, Nazbanou Nozari","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001253","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001253","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Information can be conveyed via multiple channels such as verbal and gestural (visual) channels during communication. Sometimes the information from different channels does not match (e.g., saying right while pointing to the left). How do addressees choose which information to act upon in such cases? In two experiments, we investigated this issue by having participants follow instructions on how to move objects on the screen. Experiment 1 examined whether people's choice of channel can be altered by feedback favoring either the verbal or the gestural channel. In Experiment 2, there was no feedback and participants were free to choose either channel. We also assessed participants' verbal and visuospatial working memory capacities. Results showed that, when faced with contradicting information, there is a natural bias at the group level toward relying on the verbal channel, although this bias can be temporarily altered by probabilistic feedback. Moreover, when labels were shorter and of higher frequency, participants relied more on the verbal channel. In the absence of feedback, the capacity of individuals' visual, but not verbal, working memory determined reliance on one channel versus the other. Collectively, these results show that information selection in communication is influenced by group-level biases, as well as the properties of items and characteristics of individuals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9433564","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
That means something to me: How linguistic and emotional experience affect the acquisition, representation, and processing of novel abstract concepts. 这对我来说意义重大:语言和情感体验如何影响新颖抽象概念的获取、表征和处理。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-13 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001236
Linda Espey, Marta Ghio, Christian Bellebaum, Laura Bechtold

We used a novel linguistic training paradigm to investigate the experience-dependent acquisition, representation, and processing of novel emotional and neutral abstract concepts. Participants engaged in mental imagery (n = 32) or lexico-semantic rephrasing (n = 34) of linguistic material during five training sessions and successfully learned the novel abstract concepts. Feature production after training showed that specifically emotion features enriched the emotional concepts' representations. Unexpectedly, for participants engaging in vivid mental imagery during training a higher semantic richness of the acquired emotional concepts slowed down lexical decisions. Rephrasing, in turn, promoted a better learning and processing performance than imagery, probably due to stronger established lexical associations. Our results confirm the importance of emotional and linguistic experience and additional deep lexico-semantic processing for the acquisition, representation, and processing of abstract concepts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们采用了一种新颖的语言训练范式来研究新颖的情感和中性抽象概念的习得、表征和处理对经验的依赖性。在五次训练中,受试者对语言材料进行了心理想象(32 人)或词汇语义重述(34 人),并成功学会了新的抽象概念。训练后的特征生成表明,具体的情感特征丰富了情感概念的表征。意想不到的是,对于在训练过程中进行生动心理想象的参与者来说,所获得的情感概念的语义丰富度越高,词汇决策的速度就越慢。反过来,与意象相比,复述促进了更好的学习和处理表现,这可能是由于建立了更强的词汇关联。我们的研究结果证实了情感和语言经验以及额外的深层词汇语义处理对抽象概念的习得、表征和处理的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"That means something to me: How linguistic and emotional experience affect the acquisition, representation, and processing of novel abstract concepts.","authors":"Linda Espey, Marta Ghio, Christian Bellebaum, Laura Bechtold","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001236","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001236","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We used a novel linguistic training paradigm to investigate the experience-dependent acquisition, representation, and processing of novel emotional and neutral abstract concepts. Participants engaged in mental imagery (<i>n</i> = 32) or lexico-semantic rephrasing (<i>n</i> = 34) of linguistic material during five training sessions and successfully learned the novel abstract concepts. Feature production after training showed that specifically emotion features enriched the emotional concepts' representations. Unexpectedly, for participants engaging in vivid mental imagery during training a higher semantic richness of the acquired emotional concepts slowed down lexical decisions. Rephrasing, in turn, promoted a better learning and processing performance than imagery, probably due to stronger established lexical associations. Our results confirm the importance of emotional and linguistic experience and additional deep lexico-semantic processing for the acquisition, representation, and processing of abstract concepts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9293840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1