首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
The role of spatial location in irrelevant speech revisited: A preregistered replication study. 空间位置在不相关言语中的作用:一项预先注册的重复研究。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001424
Florian Kattner, Mitra Hassanzadeh, Wolfgang Ellermeier

The goal of the present investigation was to perform a registered replication of Jones and Macken's (1995b) study, which showed that the segregation of a sequence of sounds to distinct locations reduced the disruptive effect on serial recall. Thereby, it postulated an intriguing connection between auditory stream segregation and the cognitive mechanisms underlying the irrelevant speech effect. Specifically, it was found that a sequence of changing utterances was less disruptive in stereophonic presentation, allowing each auditory object (letters) to be allocated to a unique location (right ear, left ear, center), compared to when the same sounds were played monophonically. Due to its importance for theoretical accounts of auditory distraction and because the results were somewhat equivocal, it is important to replicate this influential study with enhanced statistical power. The present replication (N = 60) confirmed that the disruptive effect of a changing-state sequence ("V-J-X") as compared to a steady-state sequence ("J-J-J")-the changing-state effect-is reduced significantly with stereophonic presentation, suggesting that listeners perceptually grouped the presented sound into three separate steady-state streams, which produce much less interference with seriation compared to the monophonic presentation. However, in contrast to the original study, stereophonic sequences tended to be slightly more disruptive than monophonic steady-state sequences, suggesting that the change in location may also cause some interference on its own. Moreover, there was also a significant steady-state effect, with both steady-state conditions being more disruptive than silence. The results are discussed with regard to interference-by-process and attentional accounts of auditory distraction. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究的目的是对Jones和Macken (1995b)的研究进行注册复制,该研究表明,将一系列声音分离到不同的位置可以减少对系列回忆的破坏性影响。因此,它假设了听觉流分离与不相关言语效应背后的认知机制之间的有趣联系。具体来说,研究发现,与单声道播放相同的声音相比,在立体声播放中,一系列变化的话语的干扰较小,允许每个听觉对象(字母)被分配到一个独特的位置(右耳,左耳,中间)。由于它对听觉分心的理论解释的重要性,并且因为结果有些模棱两可,重要的是要用增强的统计能力来复制这一有影响力的研究。目前的复制(N = 60)证实,与稳定状态序列(“J-J-J”)相比,变化状态序列(“V-J-X”)的破坏性效应(变化状态效应)在立体声呈现中显着降低,这表明听众在感知上将呈现的声音分为三个独立的稳定状态流,与单声道呈现相比,这对序列产生的干扰要小得多。然而,与最初的研究相反,立体声序列往往比单声稳态序列更具破坏性,这表明位置的变化本身也可能造成一些干扰。此外,稳态效应也很显著,两种稳态条件都比沉默更具破坏性。结果讨论了有关过程干扰和听觉分心的注意帐户。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of spatial location in irrelevant speech revisited: A preregistered replication study.","authors":"Florian Kattner, Mitra Hassanzadeh, Wolfgang Ellermeier","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The goal of the present investigation was to perform a registered replication of Jones and Macken's (1995b) study, which showed that the segregation of a sequence of sounds to distinct locations reduced the disruptive effect on serial recall. Thereby, it postulated an intriguing connection between auditory stream segregation and the cognitive mechanisms underlying the irrelevant speech effect. Specifically, it was found that a sequence of changing utterances was less disruptive in stereophonic presentation, allowing each auditory object (letters) to be allocated to a unique location (right ear, left ear, center), compared to when the same sounds were played monophonically. Due to its importance for theoretical accounts of auditory distraction and because the results were somewhat equivocal, it is important to replicate this influential study with enhanced statistical power. The present replication (N = 60) confirmed that the disruptive effect of a changing-state sequence (\"V-J-X\") as compared to a steady-state sequence (\"J-J-J\")-the changing-state effect-is reduced significantly with stereophonic presentation, suggesting that listeners perceptually grouped the presented sound into three separate steady-state streams, which produce much less interference with seriation compared to the monophonic presentation. However, in contrast to the original study, stereophonic sequences tended to be slightly more disruptive than monophonic steady-state sequences, suggesting that the change in location may also cause some interference on its own. Moreover, there was also a significant steady-state effect, with both steady-state conditions being more disruptive than silence. The results are discussed with regard to interference-by-process and attentional accounts of auditory distraction. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"50 12","pages":"1892-1900"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142973078","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phonetic retuning to idiosyncrasies in word onsets: The interplay of lexical context and prediction. 语词开始时的语音回归特质:词汇语境与预测的相互作用。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001411
Alexandra Jesse

Listeners can use both lexical context (i.e., lexical knowledge activated by the word itself) and lexical predictions based on the content of a preceding sentence to adjust their phonetic categories to speaker idiosyncrasies. However, this phonetic retuning is difficult for listeners to achieve using lexical context when adjusting to idiosyncrasies in word onsets. In this situation, sentence context could help by boosting lexical knowledge. In a series of experiments, we tested for the interplay between lexical context and sentence context. Using the sentence-guided retuning paradigm from Jesse (2021), either a preceding sentence context or following lexical context disambiguated the perceptually ambiguous onset of short words as /s/ or /f/. At test, listeners categorized steps from an /sɑ/-/fɑ/ continuum. Evidence for phonetic retuning, in terms of more responses at test in line with prior disambiguation during exposure, was found when sentence context had disambiguated the critical sound during exposure. In contrast, lexical knowledge activated by the word itself only produced trends across a subset of steps. When sentence and lexical context disambiguated the idiosyncrasy in the same words, the change of the overall retuning effect across steps followed the pattern observed in the experiment with only lexical disambiguation. Furthermore, this modulation of the retuning effect was not observed when sentence and lexical context disambiguated the idiosyncrasy in different items. This pattern of results suggests an interplay between these two types of contexts. Sentence context therefore helps with retuning to talker idiosyncrasies in word onsets when the lexical context can fail listeners. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

听者可以使用词汇语境(即由单词本身激活的词汇知识)和基于前一句内容的词汇预测来调整自己的语音类别,以适应说话者的特质。然而,这种语音回归对听者来说很难在词汇语境中实现。在这种情况下,句子语境可以帮助提高词汇知识。在一系列实验中,我们测试了词汇语境和句子语境之间的相互作用。使用Jesse(2021)的句子导向回归范式,无论是前句上下文还是后词汇上下文,都消除了/s/或/f/等短单词在感知上的歧义。在测试中,听者从/s æ /-/f æ /连续体中对步骤进行分类。当句子上下文在暴露过程中消除了关键音的歧义时,就测试中的更多反应而言,语音回归的证据被发现。相比之下,由单词本身激活的词汇知识只能在一个子集的步骤中产生趋势。当句子和词汇语境对同一词的特质进行消歧时,各步骤的整体回归效应变化遵循了仅消歧的实验模式。此外,当句子和词汇语境对不同条目的特质进行消歧时,不存在这种回调效应。这种结果模式表明这两种类型的上下文之间存在相互作用。因此,当词汇上下文可能使听者失败时,句子上下文有助于在单词开始时回归说话者的特质。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Phonetic retuning to idiosyncrasies in word onsets: The interplay of lexical context and prediction.","authors":"Alexandra Jesse","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001411","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001411","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Listeners can use both lexical context (i.e., lexical knowledge activated by the word itself) and lexical predictions based on the content of a preceding sentence to adjust their phonetic categories to speaker idiosyncrasies. However, this phonetic retuning is difficult for listeners to achieve using lexical context when adjusting to idiosyncrasies in word onsets. In this situation, sentence context could help by boosting lexical knowledge. In a series of experiments, we tested for the interplay between lexical context and sentence context. Using the sentence-guided retuning paradigm from Jesse (2021), either a preceding sentence context or following lexical context disambiguated the perceptually ambiguous onset of short words as /s/ or /f/. At test, listeners categorized steps from an /sɑ/-/fɑ/ continuum. Evidence for phonetic retuning, in terms of more responses at test in line with prior disambiguation during exposure, was found when sentence context had disambiguated the critical sound during exposure. In contrast, lexical knowledge activated by the word itself only produced trends across a subset of steps. When sentence and lexical context disambiguated the idiosyncrasy in the same words, the change of the overall retuning effect across steps followed the pattern observed in the experiment with only lexical disambiguation. Furthermore, this modulation of the retuning effect was not observed when sentence and lexical context disambiguated the idiosyncrasy in different items. This pattern of results suggests an interplay between these two types of contexts. Sentence context therefore helps with retuning to talker idiosyncrasies in word onsets when the lexical context can fail listeners. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"50 12","pages":"1918-1931"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142973076","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Coronal underspecification as an emerging property in the development of speech processing. 冠状欠规范是语音处理发展过程中的一种新特性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001367
Nadja Althaus, Aditi Lahiri, Kim Plunkett

Is the developing lexicon phonologically detailed or are representations underspecified? Experimental results from toddlers suggest phonological specificity. By contrast, the featurally underspecified lexicon theory (Lahiri, 2018; Lahiri & Reetz, 2010), motivated by evidence such as the cross-linguistic prevalence of phenomena such as coronal assimilation (rainbow → rai[m]bow), proposes that coronal sounds are unspecified for place of articulation even in the adult lexicon. The featurally underspecified lexicon, therefore, predicts that asymmetries in mispronunciation sensitivity are also present in the developing lexicon. Recent research (Ren et al., 2019) has rejected this, reporting similar sensitivity to mispronunciation of coronals and noncoronals at 19 months. Using a more sensitive experimental paradigm, we provide new evidence demonstrating a lack of asymmetries at 18 months, but mispronunciation sensitivity for coronals disappears by 24 months. In an intermodal preferential looking study, growth curve analysis shows that 18-month-olds are sensitive to mispronunciations of words with a coronal (e.g., duck vs. *buck) and noncoronal (e.g., bird vs. *dird) onset. At 24 months, mispronunciations of coronal-onset words were treated just like the accurate pronunciations. We conclude that coronals are underspecified in the developing lexicon at 24 months. We propose a model under which initial representations are phonetic in nature and require exact acoustic input, whereas phonological coronal underspecification at the lexical level emerges gradually as a result of exposure to variation in the input such as coronal assimilations that only become detectable patterns with growing lexical and segmentation skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

发育中的词汇是语音详细还是表征不明确?学步儿童的实验结果表明了语音特异性。与此相反,以冠音同化(rainbow → rai[m]bow)等跨语言现象的普遍性等证据为动机的 "发音不明确词典理论"(Lahiri, 2018; Lahiri & Reetz, 2010)提出,即使在成人词典中,冠音的发音位置也是不明确的。因此,未明确发音位置的词库预示着错误发音敏感性的不对称性也存在于发育中的词库中。最近的研究(Ren 等人,2019 年)否定了这一观点,研究报告显示,19 个月大的儿童对冠音和非冠音错误发音的敏感度相似。通过使用更敏感的实验范式,我们提供了新的证据,证明在 18 个月大时缺乏不对称现象,但对冠音的错误发音敏感性在 24 个月大时消失了。在一项模态间偏好观察研究中,成长曲线分析表明,18 个月大的儿童对冠状词(如 duck 与 *buck)和非冠状词(如 bird 与 *dird)的错误发音都很敏感。在 24 个月大时,对冠音起始词的错误发音与准确发音一样处理。我们的结论是,在 24 个月大时,冠音在正在发育的词库中还没有得到充分定义。我们提出了一个模型,即最初的表征是语音性质的,需要准确的声音输入,而词汇层面上的语音冠化不足则是随着输入的变化而逐渐出现的,如冠化同化,只有随着词汇和分词技能的不断提高,冠化同化才会成为可检测的模式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Coronal underspecification as an emerging property in the development of speech processing.","authors":"Nadja Althaus, Aditi Lahiri, Kim Plunkett","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001367","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001367","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Is the developing lexicon phonologically detailed or are representations underspecified? Experimental results from toddlers suggest phonological specificity. By contrast, the featurally underspecified lexicon theory (Lahiri, 2018; Lahiri & Reetz, 2010), motivated by evidence such as the cross-linguistic prevalence of phenomena such as coronal assimilation (rainbow → rai[m]bow), proposes that coronal sounds are unspecified for place of articulation even in the adult lexicon. The featurally underspecified lexicon, therefore, predicts that asymmetries in mispronunciation sensitivity are also present in the developing lexicon. Recent research (Ren et al., 2019) has rejected this, reporting similar sensitivity to mispronunciation of coronals and noncoronals at 19 months. Using a more sensitive experimental paradigm, we provide new evidence demonstrating a lack of asymmetries at 18 months, but mispronunciation sensitivity for coronals disappears by 24 months. In an intermodal preferential looking study, growth curve analysis shows that 18-month-olds are sensitive to mispronunciations of words with a coronal (e.g., <i>duck</i> vs. <i>*buck</i>) and noncoronal (e.g., <i>bird</i> vs<i>. *dird</i>) onset. At 24 months, mispronunciations of coronal-onset words were treated just like the accurate pronunciations. We conclude that coronals are underspecified in the developing lexicon at 24 months. We propose a model under which initial representations are phonetic in nature and require exact acoustic input, whereas phonological coronal underspecification at the lexical level emerges gradually as a result of exposure to variation in the input such as coronal assimilations that only become detectable patterns with growing lexical and segmentation skills. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1932-1953"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What makes a stimulus worthy of attention: Cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance in the learned predictiveness effect. 是什么让刺激值得关注?习得性预测效应中的线索-结果相关性和选择相关性。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001365
Jessica C Lee, Justine K Greenaway, Hilary J Don, Evan J Livesey

The learned predictiveness effect refers to the tendency for predictive cues to attract greater attention and show faster learning in subsequent tasks. However, in typical designs, the predictiveness of each cue (its objective cue-outcome correlation) is confounded with the degree to which it is informative for making the correct response on each trial (a feature we term choice relevance). In four experiments, we tested the unique contributions of cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance to the learned predictiveness effect by manipulating the outcome choices available on each trial. Experiments 1A and 1B compared two sets of partially predictive cues and found that participants learned more in a transfer phase about the set of cues that were previously choice-relevant. Experiments 2A and 2B used a design in which the cue-outcome correlation was stronger for one set of cues (perfect predictors) than the other set (imperfect predictors). Manipulating the choice relevance of the imperfect predictors in this design did not influence the magnitude of the learning bias toward the perfect predictor. Unlike cue-outcome correlation, choice relevance did not seem to correspond to biases in eye-gaze, suggesting that it operates via a distinct mechanism. Simulations with a modified EXIT model successfully predicted cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance effects by assuming that participants update learning for present outcomes only, but incorrectly predicted additive effects. We conclude that cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance are important factors that can lead to biases in future learning; both were individually sufficient but neither was necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

学习预测效应指的是预测性线索在后续任务中吸引更多注意力并表现出更快学习的趋势。然而,在典型的设计中,每个线索的预测性(其客观的线索-结果相关性)与它对每次试验做出正确反应的信息程度(我们称之为选择相关性)相混淆。在四个实验中,我们通过操纵每个试验的结果选择,测试了线索-结果相关性和选择相关性对习得性预测效应的独特贡献。实验 1A 和 1B 比较了两组部分预测性线索,发现参与者在转移阶段对之前与选择相关的那组线索学习得更多。实验 2A 和 2B 采用了一种设计,即一组线索(完美预测者)的线索-结果相关性比另一组线索(不完美预测者)更强。在这种设计中,操纵不完全预测因子的选择相关性并不会影响对完全预测因子的学习偏向程度。与线索-结果相关性不同,选择相关性似乎并不与注视偏差相对应,这表明它是通过一种不同的机制发挥作用的。通过假定参与者只对当前结果进行更新学习,使用改进的 EXIT 模型模拟成功地预测了线索-结果相关性和选择相关性效应,但却错误地预测了叠加效应。我们的结论是,提示-结果相关性和选择相关性是可能导致未来学习偏差的重要因素;二者都足以单独存在,但都不是必需的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"What makes a stimulus worthy of attention: Cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance in the learned predictiveness effect.","authors":"Jessica C Lee, Justine K Greenaway, Hilary J Don, Evan J Livesey","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001365","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001365","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The learned predictiveness effect refers to the tendency for predictive cues to attract greater attention and show faster learning in subsequent tasks. However, in typical designs, the predictiveness of each cue (its objective cue-outcome correlation) is confounded with the degree to which it is informative for making the correct response on each trial (a feature we term choice relevance). In four experiments, we tested the unique contributions of cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance to the learned predictiveness effect by manipulating the outcome choices available on each trial. Experiments 1A and 1B compared two sets of partially predictive cues and found that participants learned more in a transfer phase about the set of cues that were previously choice-relevant. Experiments 2A and 2B used a design in which the cue-outcome correlation was stronger for one set of cues (perfect predictors) than the other set (imperfect predictors). Manipulating the choice relevance of the imperfect predictors in this design did not influence the magnitude of the learning bias toward the perfect predictor. Unlike cue-outcome correlation, choice relevance did not seem to correspond to biases in eye-gaze, suggesting that it operates via a distinct mechanism. Simulations with a modified EXIT model successfully predicted cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance effects by assuming that participants update learning for present outcomes only, but incorrectly predicted additive effects. We conclude that cue-outcome correlation and choice relevance are important factors that can lead to biases in future learning; both were individually sufficient but neither was necessary. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1875-1891"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pitting base rate driven heuristics against conditional reasoning in multivariate contingency assessment. 多变量权变评估中基于条件推理的基准率驱动启发式算法。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001407
Klaus Fiedler, Florian Kutzner

Contingency assessment is a major module of adaptive cognition and a prominent topic of ecological rationality. Virtually all influential theories assume that contingency estimates between Y and X are inferred from subjective conditional probabilities of focal Y levels given different X levels, p ( Y focal | X different levels ) . Yet, conditional probabilities are cognitively demanding, as Yfocal must be assessed separately for all levels of Xdifferent level. Pseudocontingencies (PCs) afford an alternative mechanism relying on base rates. In a PC, the more frequent level on one attribute appears contingent on the more frequent level on another attribute. When PCs are manipulated orthogonally to conditional probabilities, the former dominate the latter (Fiedler, 2010). PC dominance is shown in Experiments 1 and 1a to be particularly striking when a multivariate task setting calls for the assessment of all k·(k - 1)/2 pairwise contingencies between k attributes. Experiment 2 shows that contingency judgments are dissociated from evaluative conditioning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

权变评价是适应性认知的一个重要模块,也是生态理性的一个突出课题。几乎所有有影响力的理论都假设,Y和X之间的权变估计是从给定不同X水平的Y震源水平的主观条件概率p (Y震源| X不同水平)推断出来的。然而,条件概率是认知上的要求,因为Yfocal必须单独评估所有级别的x不同级别。伪应急(PCs)提供了一种依赖于基准利率的替代机制。在PC中,一个属性的更频繁级别似乎取决于另一个属性的更频繁级别。当pc与条件概率正交操作时,前者支配后者(Fiedler, 2010)。在实验1和1a中显示,当一个多变量任务设置要求评估k个属性之间的所有k·(k - 1)/2成对偶然性时,PC优势尤其引人注目。实验2表明,权变判断与评价条件作用是分离的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Pitting base rate driven heuristics against conditional reasoning in multivariate contingency assessment.","authors":"Klaus Fiedler, Florian Kutzner","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001407","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001407","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Contingency assessment is a major module of adaptive cognition and a prominent topic of ecological rationality. Virtually all influential theories assume that contingency estimates between Y and X are inferred from subjective conditional probabilities of focal Y levels given different X levels, p ( Y focal | X different levels ) . Yet, conditional probabilities are cognitively demanding, as Yfocal must be assessed separately for all levels of Xdifferent level. Pseudocontingencies (PCs) afford an alternative mechanism relying on base rates. In a PC, the more frequent level on one attribute appears contingent on the more frequent level on another attribute. When PCs are manipulated orthogonally to conditional probabilities, the former dominate the latter (Fiedler, 2010). PC dominance is shown in Experiments 1 and 1a to be particularly striking when a multivariate task setting calls for the assessment of all k·(k - 1)/2 pairwise contingencies between k attributes. Experiment 2 shows that contingency judgments are dissociated from evaluative conditioning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":"50 12","pages":"1901-1917"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142973077","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Marking prosodic prominence for voice assistant and human addressees. 为语音助手和人类受话人标注前音突出度
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001396
Eleonora Beier, Michelle Cohn, Timothy Trammel, Fernanda Ferreira, Georgia Zellou

Prosodic prominence (realized with phonetic features such as increased intensity, duration, and pitch, among others) is thought to guide listeners' attention by focusing new information. This study investigates production and perception of prosodic prominence toward two types of addressees: a human and a voice assistant interlocutor. We examine how the language system adapts to this increasingly common technology, by testing whether prosodic prominence is subject to audience design when addressing an interlocutor that is consistently rated as having less communicative ability. Stimuli consisted of question-answer pairs, where California English speakers read identical sentences (e.g., "Jude saw the sun") in response to interlocutors' questions probing different foci (e.g., "Who saw the sun?"). Experiment 1 reveals consistent acoustic adjustments to mark focus on either the subject or the object of a sentence. In Experiment 2, we find that listeners reliably infer the intended information structure based on these acoustic adjustments. Across both experiments, we see no consistent difference in focus marking by type of interlocutor (human vs. voice assistant). Nonetheless, listeners associate particular features (slower speech rate) with speech directed at voice assistants. Taken together, our findings suggest that while speakers apply communicative strategies from human-human interaction when addressing voice assistants, listeners expect a device-specific register. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们认为,前音突出(通过增加强度、持续时间和音高等语音特征来实现)可以通过集中新信息来引导听者的注意力。本研究调查了两种类型的受话者对前音突出的产生和感知:人类和语音助理对话者。我们检验了语言系统如何适应这种日益普遍的技术,并测试了在向一直被评为交际能力较弱的对话者说话时,前音突出是否受受众设计的影响。刺激物包括问答对,加州英语使用者在回答对话者提出的不同焦点问题(如 "谁看到了太阳")时,要朗读相同的句子(如 "Jude saw the sun")。实验 1 显示了一致的声学调整,以标记句子中的主语或宾语。在实验 2 中,我们发现听者能根据这些声音调整可靠地推断出预期的信息结构。在这两项实验中,我们发现不同类型的对话者(人类与语音助手)在重点标记方面没有一致的差异。然而,听者会将特定的特征(语速较慢)与针对语音助手的语音联系起来。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,虽然说话者在对语音助手讲话时采用了人与人之间的交流策略,但听者却期望使用特定设备的语域。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Marking prosodic prominence for voice assistant and human addressees.","authors":"Eleonora Beier, Michelle Cohn, Timothy Trammel, Fernanda Ferreira, Georgia Zellou","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001396","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001396","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prosodic prominence (realized with phonetic features such as increased intensity, duration, and pitch, among others) is thought to guide listeners' attention by focusing new information. This study investigates production and perception of prosodic prominence toward two types of addressees: a human and a voice assistant interlocutor. We examine how the language system adapts to this increasingly common technology, by testing whether prosodic prominence is subject to <i>audience design</i> when addressing an interlocutor that is consistently rated as having less communicative ability. Stimuli consisted of question-answer pairs, where California English speakers read identical sentences (e.g., \"Jude saw the sun\") in response to interlocutors' questions probing different foci (e.g., \"Who saw the sun?\"). Experiment 1 reveals consistent acoustic adjustments to mark focus on either the subject or the object of a sentence. In Experiment 2, we find that listeners reliably infer the intended information structure based on these acoustic adjustments. Across both experiments, we see no consistent difference in focus marking by type of interlocutor (human vs. voice assistant). Nonetheless, listeners associate particular features (slower speech rate) with speech directed at voice assistants. Taken together, our findings suggest that while speakers apply communicative strategies from human-human interaction when addressing voice assistants, listeners expect a device-specific register. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142649418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct and indirect effects of fluid intelligence on the retrieval practice effect. 流体智力对检索练习效应的直接和间接影响。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-14 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001401
Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto

In this study, participants (N = 144) first studied 40 word pairs, then restudied half of the word pairs and practiced retrieval with feedback on the other half. In separate sessions, they then completed cued-recall and fluid intelligence (gF) tests. Three main objectives were addressed. First, we sought to generalize two findings reported by M. Minear et al. (2018): During the final-test phase, the high gF group exhibited a greater retrieval practice effect for difficult items compared to easy items, while the opposite pattern was observed for the low gF group; and, during the practice phase, the advantage of the high gF group over the low gF group increased across cycles for difficult items but not for easy items. Overall, we successfully extended their results. Second, we investigated whether gF is related to the amount of new items recalled during the practice phase. Consistent positive relationships were found in Cycles 1-3 (rs between .30 and .43). Third, we tested and found an indirect effect of gF on the retrieval practice effect mediated by performance during the practice phase. One possibility is that learners with higher gF may be particularly skilled at generating effective mediators and at monitoring and replacing less effective ones after retrieval failures. We recommend the following research agenda: measure the production, shift, and retrieval of mediators; manipulate the number of retrieval practice opportunities; probe the retrieval practice effect with free-recall tests; and adopt procedures based on learning to a criterion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在这项研究中,参与者(144 人)首先学习了 40 个词对,然后重新学习了一半的词对,并在另一半词对的反馈下进行了检索练习。然后,他们在不同的环节中分别完成了提示记忆和流体智力(gF)测试。我们的目标主要有三个。首先,我们试图推广 M. Minear 等人(2018 年)报告的两项发现:在最终测试阶段,与简单项目相比,高gF组在困难项目上表现出更大的检索练习效应,而低gF组则观察到相反的模式;在练习阶段,高gF组与低gF组相比,在困难项目上的优势在不同周期都会增加,但在简单项目上则不会。总之,我们成功地扩展了他们的结果。其次,我们研究了 gF 是否与练习阶段回忆的新项目数量有关。在第 1-3 周期中发现了一致的正相关关系(rs 在 0.30 和 0.43 之间)。第三,我们测试并发现了 gF 对检索练习效果的间接影响,这种影响是由练习阶段的成绩所中介的。一种可能性是,gF 较高的学习者可能特别善于产生有效的中介物,并在检索失败后监控和替换效果较差的中介物。我们建议以下研究议程:测量中介物的产生、转移和检索;操纵检索练习机会的数量;通过自由回忆测试探究检索练习效应;以及采用基于学习标准的程序。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Direct and indirect effects of fluid intelligence on the retrieval practice effect.","authors":"Marcos Felipe Rodrigues de Lima, Luciano Grüdtner Buratto","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, participants (<i>N</i> = 144) first studied 40 word pairs, then restudied half of the word pairs and practiced retrieval with feedback on the other half. In separate sessions, they then completed cued-recall and fluid intelligence (gF) tests. Three main objectives were addressed. First, we sought to generalize two findings reported by M. Minear et al. (2018): During the final-test phase, the high gF group exhibited a greater retrieval practice effect for difficult items compared to easy items, while the opposite pattern was observed for the low gF group; and, during the practice phase, the advantage of the high gF group over the low gF group increased across cycles for difficult items but not for easy items. Overall, we successfully extended their results. Second, we investigated whether gF is related to the amount of new items recalled during the practice phase. Consistent positive relationships were found in Cycles 1-3 (<i>r</i>s between .30 and .43). Third, we tested and found an indirect effect of gF on the retrieval practice effect mediated by performance during the practice phase. One possibility is that learners with higher gF may be particularly skilled at generating effective mediators and at monitoring and replacing less effective ones after retrieval failures. We recommend the following research agenda: measure the production, shift, and retrieval of mediators; manipulate the number of retrieval practice opportunities; probe the retrieval practice effect with free-recall tests; and adopt procedures based on learning to a criterion. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142631795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When confidence reveals more than recognition performance does: The case of context load. 当信心比识别成绩更能说明问题时:语境负荷案例
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001391
Oliwia Zaborowska, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Katarzyna Zawadzka, Maciej Hanczakowski

Context in which events are embedded is often hypothesized to serve as an independent cue for retrieval. This means that any effects of context need to obey two basic principles of cue-dependent memory: Memory retrieval should be augmented when, first, encoding context is reinstated and, second, this context uniquely specifies individual items stored in memory. Both of these regularities are well supported for recall tests, but they remain contentious in recognition tests. Here, in three experiments, we assess whether unique and nonunique contexts affect memory processes when reinstated during recognition. However, rather than focusing on measures of recognition performance, we looked at confidence judgments collected during recognition that should be particularly sensitive to recollective effects resulting from context cuing. Experiments 1 and 2, using old/new and forced-choice recognition tests, respectively, documented positive effects of context reinstatement on confidence in correct recognition identifications, but only for contexts uniquely associated with individual items. These effects emerged even when there were no reliable context effects in recognition performance measures. Experiment 3 showed the same effect of context reinstatement, moderated by context load, when spontaneous recognition of a previous study episode occurred during restudy. These results demonstrate the role of context as an independent retrieval cue both in deliberate and spontaneous recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

嵌入事件的语境通常被假定为检索的独立线索。这意味着语境的任何影响都需要遵守线索依赖记忆的两个基本原则:首先,当编码情境被恢复时,记忆检索应该会增强;其次,当这种情境唯一指定了存储在记忆中的单个项目时,记忆检索应该会增强。这两个规律在回忆测试中都得到了很好的支持,但在识别测试中却仍然存在争议。在这里,我们通过三个实验来评估独特和非独特的语境在识别过程中被恢复时是否会影响记忆过程。不过,我们并没有把重点放在识别成绩的测量上,而是研究了在识别过程中收集到的信心判断,这种判断应该对情境诱导导致的回忆效应特别敏感。实验一和实验二分别使用了新旧识别测试和强迫选择识别测试,结果表明情境恢复对正确识别的信心有积极影响,但仅限于与单个项目唯一相关的情境。即使在识别成绩测量中没有可靠的情境效应时,这些效应也会出现。实验 3 显示,当在复习过程中自发识别以前的学习情节时,情境恢复也会产生同样的效应,但会受到情境负荷的调节。这些结果证明了情境在有意识别和自发识别中作为独立检索线索的作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"When confidence reveals more than recognition performance does: The case of context load.","authors":"Oliwia Zaborowska, Beatrice G Kuhlmann, Katarzyna Zawadzka, Maciej Hanczakowski","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001391","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001391","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Context in which events are embedded is often hypothesized to serve as an independent cue for retrieval. This means that any effects of context need to obey two basic principles of cue-dependent memory: Memory retrieval should be augmented when, first, encoding context is reinstated and, second, this context uniquely specifies individual items stored in memory. Both of these regularities are well supported for recall tests, but they remain contentious in recognition tests. Here, in three experiments, we assess whether unique and nonunique contexts affect memory processes when reinstated during recognition. However, rather than focusing on measures of recognition performance, we looked at confidence judgments collected during recognition that should be particularly sensitive to recollective effects resulting from context cuing. Experiments 1 and 2, using old/new and forced-choice recognition tests, respectively, documented positive effects of context reinstatement on confidence in correct recognition identifications, but only for contexts uniquely associated with individual items. These effects emerged even when there were no reliable context effects in recognition performance measures. Experiment 3 showed the same effect of context reinstatement, moderated by context load, when spontaneous recognition of a previous study episode occurred during restudy. These results demonstrate the role of context as an independent retrieval cue both in deliberate and spontaneous recognition. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1722-1739"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bilingual parafoveal processing: Children and adults preprocess orthographic information of the upcoming word during sentence reading in their first and second language. 双语视网膜旁处理:儿童和成人在用第一语言和第二语言阅读句子时,对即将出现的单词的正字法信息进行预处理。
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001346
Simon P Tiffin-Richards

Readers of different ages and across different languages routinely process information of upcoming words in a sentence, before their eyes move to fixate them directly (parafoveal processing). However, there is inconsistent evidence of similar parafoveal processing in a reader's second language (L2). In this eye movement study, the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975a) was used to test whether parafoveal processing of orthographic information is an integral part of both beginning and proficient L2 reading. The eye movements of beginning L2-learners (n = 53, aged 11-14 years) and highly proficient L2-users (n = 56, aged 19-65 years) were recorded while they read sentences in their first language (L1) German and L2 English. Sentences each contained a cognate target word (e.g., English: tunnel, German: Tunnel). The parafoveal preview of the targets either (a) preserved the spelling and meaning of the target (identity condition), (b) preserved letter identities but transposed the position of two adjacent letters (transposed-letter [TL] condition, e.g., tunenl/Tunenl), or substituted the identity of two adjacent letters (substituted-letter condition, e.g., tunocl/Tunocl). TL previews elicited longer early first-pass reading times than identity previews in both L1 and L2 reading in children and adults, suggesting that letter position was processed parafoveally. Substituted-letter previews resulted in longer reading times than TL previews in children and adults in L1 and L2, suggesting that letter identity information was processed independently of position information. These results suggest that letter position and identity information are extracted from the parafovea during L1 and L2 reading, facilitating word recognition in children and adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

不同年龄段和不同语言的读者在眼睛直接注视句子中即将出现的单词之前,都会对这些单词的信息进行常规处理(视网膜旁处理)。然而,在读者的第二语言(L2)中也存在类似的视线旁处理,但证据并不一致。在这项眼动研究中,我们采用了凝视--权变边界范式(Rayner,1975a)来测试正字法信息的视线旁处理是否是初学和精通第二语言阅读不可或缺的一部分。研究人员记录了初学 L2 学习者(n = 53,年龄 11-14 岁)和精通 L2 学习者(n = 56,年龄 19-65 岁)在阅读第一语言(L1)德语和第二语言英语句子时的眼球运动。每个句子都包含一个同义目标词(例如,英语:隧道,德语:隧道)。目标词的视网膜旁预览要么(a)保留目标词的拼写和含义(同一性条件),要么(b)保留字母同一性,但调换两个相邻字母的位置(调换字母[TL]条件,如 tunenl/Tunenl),要么(调换字母条件,如 tunocl/Tunocl)替换两个相邻字母的同一性。在儿童和成人的 L1 和 L2 阅读中,TL 预览比同一性预览引起的早期一读时间更长,这表明字母位置是在视网膜旁处理的。在儿童和成人的 L1 和 L2 阅读中,替换字母预览比 TL 预览的阅读时间更长,这表明字母身份信息的处理与位置信息无关。这些结果表明,在 L1 和 L2 阅读过程中,字母位置和特征信息是从视网膜旁提取的,这有助于儿童和成人的单词识别。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bilingual parafoveal processing: Children and adults preprocess orthographic information of the upcoming word during sentence reading in their first and second language.","authors":"Simon P Tiffin-Richards","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001346","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001346","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Readers of different ages and across different languages routinely process information of upcoming words in a sentence, before their eyes move to fixate them directly (parafoveal processing). However, there is inconsistent evidence of similar parafoveal processing in a reader's second language (L2). In this eye movement study, the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975a) was used to test whether parafoveal processing of orthographic information is an integral part of both beginning and proficient L2 reading. The eye movements of beginning L2-learners (<i>n</i> = 53, aged 11-14 years) and highly proficient L2-users (<i>n</i> = 56, aged 19-65 years) were recorded while they read sentences in their first language (L1) German and L2 English. Sentences each contained a cognate target word (e.g., English: tunnel, German: Tunnel). The parafoveal preview of the targets either (a) preserved the spelling and meaning of the target (identity condition), (b) preserved letter identities but transposed the position of two adjacent letters (transposed-letter [TL] condition, e.g., tunenl/Tunenl), or substituted the identity of two adjacent letters (substituted-letter condition, e.g., tunocl/Tunocl). TL previews elicited longer early first-pass reading times than identity previews in both L1 and L2 reading in children and adults, suggesting that letter position was processed parafoveally. Substituted-letter previews resulted in longer reading times than TL previews in children and adults in L1 and L2, suggesting that letter identity information was processed independently of position information. These results suggest that letter position and identity information are extracted from the parafovea during L1 and L2 reading, facilitating word recognition in children and adults. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1844-1861"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140854406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms. 人脸竞赛对元记忆的影响:研究其稳健性和内在机制
IF 2.2 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001392
Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata

Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

知觉者对同种族面孔的识别记忆通常优于对跨种族面孔的识别记忆,这种现象被称为跨种族效应(CRE)。尽管跨种族效应无处不在,但人们是否在元认知上意识到跨种族效应尚不清楚。本研究通过五项实验深入调查了感知者对 CRE 的元认知意识。实验 1 和 2 表明,前瞻性(学习判断)和回顾性(信心)元认知判断对人脸的种族类别和原型的变化都很敏感。实验 3 表明,受试者的项目级前瞻性判断受到了关于人脸种族对记忆表现影响的信念的影响。实验 4 显示,在没有直接刺激经验的情况下,全局预测会受到人脸种族的影响,这强调了预先存在的信念的作用。实验 5 扩展了这些发现,在全局预测和项目层面的前瞻性判断中,人脸种族和参与者种族之间都有很大的交叉互动,表明白人和黑人参与者对内群体人脸都有较高的元记忆估计。该实验进一步表明,预先存在的信念会加强人脸种族对元记忆判断的影响,但并不能完全解释这种影响。总之,这些实验提供了有力的证据,证明白人参与者对不同种族类别和原型性的面孔具有良好的元记忆准确性,并证明信念是面孔种族对白人和黑人参与者元记忆判断影响的基础,尽管这可能不是唯一的机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of face race on metamemory: Examining its robustness and underlying mechanisms.","authors":"Tomás A Palma, Alexandre Vieira, Francisco Cruz, André Mata","doi":"10.1037/xlm0001392","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xlm0001392","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Perceivers typically exhibit better recognition memory for same-race faces than for cross-race faces, a phenomenon known as the cross-race effect (CRE). Despite its ubiquity, it is yet unclear whether people are metacognitively aware of the CRE. This research thoroughly investigates perceivers' metacognitive awareness of the CRE across five experiments. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that both prospective (judgments of learning) and retrospective (confidence) metamemory judgments are sensitive to variations in the racial category and prototypicality of faces. Experiment 3 showed that participants' item-level prospective judgments are informed by beliefs about the impact of face race on memory performance. Experiment 4 revealed that global predictions are influenced by face race in the absence of direct stimulus experience, emphasizing the role of preexisting beliefs. Experiment 5 extended these findings by showing large crossover interactions between face race and participant race in both global predictions and item-level prospective judgments, indicating that both White and Black participants have higher metamemory estimates for ingroup faces. This experiment further showed that preexisting beliefs intensify the impact of face race on metamemory judgments yet do not fully account for it. Collectively, these experiments provide robust evidence of good metamemory accuracy for faces varying in racial categories and prototypicality among White participants and demonstrate that beliefs underlie the effect of face race on metamemory judgments among both White and Black participants, though this may not be the only mechanism involved. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":50194,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1811-1843"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142114321","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1