首页 > 最新文献

Language and Speech最新文献

英文 中文
Prosodic Cues for Broad, Narrow, and Corrective Focus in Persian.
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-01 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241311924
Mortaza Taheri-Ardali, Simon Roessig, Lena Pagel, Doris Mücke

Previous studies have demonstrated that focus significantly alters sentential prosody in Persian. However, research on the phonetic realization of non-corrective narrow focus is scarce compared to that on broad and corrective focus. This paper presents a systematic production study investigating whether Persian speakers distinguish between three focus structures on target words that bear a pitch accent, that is, broad, narrow, and corrective focus. In a multidimensional phonetic analysis, we investigated the parameters of intensity, duration, and F0. Taking a local perspective, results show that the duration of the target word is a robust cue for focus marking in both syllables of the word, exhibiting a three-step pattern (corrective > narrow > broad). In the first syllable, intensity is a reliable cue to distinguish broad focus from the other two focus types, with higher intensities in broad focus. In the accented syllable, a different two-step pattern is observed, with narrow and corrective focus showing larger F0 spans than broad focus. Taking a global perspective that considers the parts of the utterance before and after the target word, we find a lowering of F0 and decreased intensity for narrow and corrective focus in the pre-target region. In the post-target region, we find strong evidence for differences in mean F0 and intensity with lower F0 in corrective focus than in broad and narrow focus, while the intensity is lower in narrow and corrective focus than in broad focus. Our analysis deepens our understanding of Persian prosody.

{"title":"Prosodic Cues for Broad, Narrow, and Corrective Focus in Persian.","authors":"Mortaza Taheri-Ardali, Simon Roessig, Lena Pagel, Doris Mücke","doi":"10.1177/00238309241311924","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241311924","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have demonstrated that focus significantly alters sentential prosody in Persian. However, research on the phonetic realization of non-corrective narrow focus is scarce compared to that on broad and corrective focus. This paper presents a systematic production study investigating whether Persian speakers distinguish between three focus structures on target words that bear a pitch accent, that is, broad, narrow, and corrective focus. In a multidimensional phonetic analysis, we investigated the parameters of intensity, duration, and F0. Taking a local perspective, results show that the duration of the target word is a robust cue for focus marking in both syllables of the word, exhibiting a three-step pattern (corrective > narrow > broad). In the first syllable, intensity is a reliable cue to distinguish broad focus from the other two focus types, with higher intensities in broad focus. In the accented syllable, a different two-step pattern is observed, with narrow and corrective focus showing larger F0 spans than broad focus. Taking a global perspective that considers the parts of the utterance before and after the target word, we find a lowering of F0 and decreased intensity for narrow and corrective focus in the pre-target region. In the post-target region, we find strong evidence for differences in mean F0 and intensity with lower F0 in corrective focus than in broad and narrow focus, while the intensity is lower in narrow and corrective focus than in broad focus. Our analysis deepens our understanding of Persian prosody.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241311924"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143076443","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Systematicity on Word Learning in Preschool Children: The Case of Semitic Morpho-Phonology.
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-29 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241312983
Niveen Omar, Bracha Nir, Karen Banai

This study investigated the role of systematicity in word learning, focusing on Semitic morpho-phonology where words exhibit multiple levels of systematicity. Building upon previous research on phonological templates, we explored how systematicity based on such templates, whether they encode meanings or not, influenced word learning in preschool-age Hebrew-speaking children. We examined form-meaning systematicity, where words share phonological templates and carry similar categorical meanings of manner-of-motion (e.g., finupál and bizudáx carry the meaning of skipping), and form-only systematicity, where words are phonologically similar but do not share a meaning (e.g., finupál and bizudáx belong to different categories of manner-of-motion). We aimed to discern how these systematicity types impact the learning of the meaning of the word as a whole, that is, the encoding of visual form combined with manner-of-motion. Using novel Semitic-like stimuli, our experiments demonstrated that different types of systematicity involve different effects on word learning. Experiment 1 showed that form-meaning systematicity hindered the learning of the manner-of-motion. In contrast, Experiment 2 revealed that form systematicity facilitated learning these features. The findings suggest a complex interplay of top-down and bottom-up processes in word learning, expanding our understanding of systematicity in word learning.

{"title":"Effects of Systematicity on Word Learning in Preschool Children: The Case of Semitic Morpho-Phonology.","authors":"Niveen Omar, Bracha Nir, Karen Banai","doi":"10.1177/00238309241312983","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241312983","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated the role of systematicity in word learning, focusing on Semitic morpho-phonology where words exhibit multiple levels of systematicity. Building upon previous research on phonological templates, we explored how systematicity based on such templates, whether they encode meanings or not, influenced word learning in preschool-age Hebrew-speaking children. We examined form-meaning systematicity, where words share phonological templates and carry similar categorical meanings of manner-of-motion (e.g., <i>finupál</i> and <i>bizudáx</i> carry the meaning of skipping), and form-only systematicity, where words are phonologically similar but do not share a meaning (e.g., <i>finupál</i> and <i>bizudáx</i> belong to different categories of manner-of-motion). We aimed to discern how these systematicity types impact the learning of the meaning of the word as a whole, that is, the encoding of visual form combined with manner-of-motion. Using novel Semitic-like stimuli, our experiments demonstrated that different types of systematicity involve different effects on word learning. Experiment 1 showed that form-meaning systematicity hindered the learning of the manner-of-motion. In contrast, Experiment 2 revealed that form systematicity facilitated learning these features. The findings suggest a complex interplay of top-down and bottom-up processes in word learning, expanding our understanding of systematicity in word learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241312983"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143061395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Templatic Is Arabic Input to Children? The Role of Child-Directed-Speech in the Acquisition of Semitic Morpho-Phonology.
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241311230
Ghada Khattab, Tamar Keren-Portnoy

Semitic languages such as Hebrew and Arabic are known for having a non-concatenative morphology: words are typically built of a combination of a consonantal root, typically tri-consonantal (e.g., k-t-b "related to writing" in Modern Standard Arabic (MSA)), with a prosodic template. Research on Hebrew language development suggests early sensitivity to frequently occurring templates. For the Arabic dialects, little is known about whether implicit sensitivity to non-concatenative morphology develops at a young age through exposure to speech, and how templatic the spoken language is in comparison to MSA. We focus on Lebanese Arabic. We hypothesized that prolonged contact with French and English may have "diluted" the salience of roots and patterns in the input. We used three different corpora of adult-directed-speech (ADS), child-directed-speech (CDS), and child speech. We analyzed the root and pattern structures in the 50 most frequent Lebanese Arabic word types in each corpus. We found fewer words with templatic patterns than expected among the most frequent words in ADS (35/50), even fewer in CDS (23/50) and still fewer in the children's target words (15/50). In addition, only a minority contains three root consonants in their surface forms: 22 in ADS, 15 in CDS, and only 7 in words targeted by the children. We conclude that Semitic structure is less evident in either input to children or words targeted by children aged 1-3 than has been assumed. We discuss implications for the development of sensitivity to templatic structure among Lebanese-acquiring children.

{"title":"How Templatic Is Arabic Input to Children? The Role of Child-Directed-Speech in the Acquisition of Semitic Morpho-Phonology.","authors":"Ghada Khattab, Tamar Keren-Portnoy","doi":"10.1177/00238309241311230","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241311230","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Semitic languages such as Hebrew and Arabic are known for having a non-concatenative morphology: words are typically built of a combination of a consonantal root, typically tri-consonantal (e.g., k-t-b \"related to writing\" in Modern Standard Arabic (MSA)), with a prosodic template. Research on Hebrew language development suggests early sensitivity to frequently occurring templates. For the Arabic dialects, little is known about whether implicit sensitivity to non-concatenative morphology develops at a young age through exposure to speech, and how templatic the spoken language is in comparison to MSA. We focus on Lebanese Arabic. We hypothesized that prolonged contact with French and English may have \"diluted\" the salience of roots and patterns in the input. We used three different corpora of adult-directed-speech (ADS), child-directed-speech (CDS), and child speech. We analyzed the root and pattern structures in the 50 most frequent Lebanese Arabic word types in each corpus. We found fewer words with templatic patterns than expected among the most frequent words in ADS (35/50), even fewer in CDS (23/50) and still fewer in the children's target words (15/50). In addition, only a minority contains three root consonants in their surface forms: 22 in ADS, 15 in CDS, and only 7 in words targeted by the children. We conclude that Semitic structure is less evident in either input to children or words targeted by children aged 1-3 than has been assumed. We discuss implications for the development of sensitivity to templatic structure among Lebanese-acquiring children.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241311230"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143025716","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preference for Distinct Variants in Learning Sound Correspondences During Dialect Acquisition. 方言习得过程中不同变体对语音对应学习的偏好。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-15 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241308171
Xiaoyu Yu, Youngah Do

Sound correspondences (SCs) have been found to be learnable phonological patterns in second dialect acquisition. Cross-linguistically, SCs consist of similar as well as distinct variants. However, in the study of SC learning, the effect of the similarity between the corresponding variants remains understudied. The salience hypothesis proposes that distinct dialect variants are more salient and learnable, while the learning bias hypothesis in phonological learning predicts that SC patterns with similar variants are preferred by learners. We conducted an artificial language learning experiment to test how sound similarity affects SC learning. Specifically, the degrees of similarity between variants were evaluated from multidimensional metrics, including phonetic and phonological measures, which are cross-validated with typological evidence. While there was no effect of variant similarity in learning simple one-to-one SCs, a preference for the most distinct dialect variant was found in the learning of SCs exhibiting more complex mapping structures (i.e., two-to-one and one-to-two). Our results confirm a preference for distinct variants in SC learning, although this effect relies on two conditions. First, the preference for distinction emerges only in the presence of complex mapping structures. Second, this preference requires an activation threshold, in that the distance of the SC must be sufficiently large to trigger the effect.

语音对应是第二方言习得过程中可习得的语音模式。跨语言,SCs既有相似的变体,也有不同的变体。然而,在SC学习的研究中,相应变体之间的相似性的影响仍未得到充分的研究。显著性假说认为,不同的方言变体更显著,更容易学习,而语音学习中的学习偏差假说则预测,具有相似变体的SC模式更容易被学习者所偏好。我们进行了一个人工语言学习实验来测试声音相似性如何影响SC学习。具体而言,变体之间的相似程度是通过多维度量来评估的,包括语音和音系测量,这些度量与类型学证据交叉验证。虽然变体相似性在学习简单的一对一方言中没有影响,但在学习具有更复杂映射结构(即二比一和一对二)的方言变体时,发现了对最独特方言变体的偏好。我们的研究结果证实了SC学习中对不同变体的偏好,尽管这种影响依赖于两个条件。首先,只有在存在复杂的映射结构时,才会出现对区别的偏好。其次,这种偏好需要一个激活阈值,因为SC的距离必须足够大才能触发这种效果。
{"title":"Preference for Distinct Variants in Learning Sound Correspondences During Dialect Acquisition.","authors":"Xiaoyu Yu, Youngah Do","doi":"10.1177/00238309241308171","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241308171","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sound correspondences (SCs) have been found to be learnable phonological patterns in second dialect acquisition. Cross-linguistically, SCs consist of similar as well as distinct variants. However, in the study of SC learning, the effect of the similarity between the corresponding variants remains understudied. The salience hypothesis proposes that distinct dialect variants are more salient and learnable, while the learning bias hypothesis in phonological learning predicts that SC patterns with similar variants are preferred by learners. We conducted an artificial language learning experiment to test how sound similarity affects SC learning. Specifically, the degrees of similarity between variants were evaluated from multidimensional metrics, including phonetic and phonological measures, which are cross-validated with typological evidence. While there was no effect of variant similarity in learning simple one-to-one SCs, a preference for the most distinct dialect variant was found in the learning of SCs exhibiting more complex mapping structures (i.e., two-to-one and one-to-two). Our results confirm a preference for distinct variants in SC learning, although this effect relies on two conditions. First, the preference for distinction emerges only in the presence of complex mapping structures. Second, this preference requires an activation threshold, in that the distance of the SC must be sufficiently large to trigger the effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241308171"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142985381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Politeness and Prosody: The Effect of Power, Distance, and Imposition on Pitch Contours in Spanish. 礼貌与韵律:权力、距离和强加对西班牙语音高轮廓的影响。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241307891
Bruno Staszkiewicz

Research in the last few decades has examined the intersection between phonetics and politeness in multiple languages. While most of the studies have analyzed the role of politeness on suprasegmental features (i.e., pitch or duration), few have considered the key contextual variables of power, distance, and imposition. This study investigates the systematic effects of power, distance, and imposition on the final intonational contours of polar questions in Central Peninsular Spanish native speakers. A total of 36 native speakers from Madrid completed a contextualized reading-sentence task in which they read aloud paragraph-length contextualizing situations and the target polar questions. The situations were balanced for two levels of power (high/low), distance (high/low), and imposition (high/low). The results from the contextualized reading-sentence task showed that the low-rising final intonational contour (L*H%) was the most employed intonational contour in every context, while the remaining contours were H*H%, H*L%, L*L%. The results confirm that L*H% is the prevailing final intonational contour in Spanish polar questions while also shedding light on the variability of other intonational configurations. In addition, the study determines whether final nuclear contours are impacted by power, distance, and impositions. The findings are discussed within the framework of Politeness Theory and the work on the phonetics and pragmatics interface.

过去几十年的研究考察了多种语言中语音和礼貌之间的交集。虽然大多数研究都分析了礼貌在超片段特征(即音调或持续时间)上的作用,但很少有人考虑到权力、距离和强加等关键的语境变量。本研究调查了权力、距离和强加对中半岛西班牙语母语者极地问题最终语调轮廓的系统影响。来自马德里的36名母语人士完成了一项语境化的句子阅读任务,在这项任务中,他们大声朗读段落的语境化情景和目标极性问题。在权力(高/低)、距离(高/低)和强制(高/低)两个层面上,情况是平衡的。语境化阅读句子任务结果显示,低升语调轮廓(L*H%)是各语境中使用最多的语调轮廓,其余语调轮廓分别为H*H%、H*L%、L*L%。结果证实,L*H%是西班牙语极性问题中普遍存在的最终语调轮廓线,同时也揭示了其他语调配置的可变性。此外,该研究还确定了最终的核轮廓是否受到功率、距离和施加的影响。研究结果在礼貌理论和语音语用界面研究的框架内进行了讨论。
{"title":"Politeness and Prosody: The Effect of Power, Distance, and Imposition on Pitch Contours in Spanish.","authors":"Bruno Staszkiewicz","doi":"10.1177/00238309241307891","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241307891","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research in the last few decades has examined the intersection between phonetics and politeness in multiple languages. While most of the studies have analyzed the role of politeness on suprasegmental features (i.e., pitch or duration), few have considered the key contextual variables of power, distance, and imposition. This study investigates the systematic effects of power, distance, and imposition on the final intonational contours of polar questions in Central Peninsular Spanish native speakers. A total of 36 native speakers from Madrid completed a contextualized reading-sentence task in which they read aloud paragraph-length contextualizing situations and the target polar questions. The situations were balanced for two levels of power (high/low), distance (high/low), and imposition (high/low). The results from the contextualized reading-sentence task showed that the low-rising final intonational contour (L*H%) was the most employed intonational contour in every context, while the remaining contours were H*H%, H*L%, L*L%. The results confirm that L*H% is the prevailing final intonational contour in Spanish polar questions while also shedding light on the variability of other intonational configurations. In addition, the study determines whether final nuclear contours are impacted by power, distance, and impositions. The findings are discussed within the framework of Politeness Theory and the work on the phonetics and pragmatics interface.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241307891"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142933489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prosodic Modifications to Challenging Communicative Environments in Preschoolers. 学龄前儿童挑战性交际环境中的韵律修饰。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-01-04 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241306748
Hoyoung Yi, Delaney DiCristofaro, Woonyoung Song

Adapting one's speaking style is particularly crucial as children start interacting with diverse conversational partners in various communication contexts. The study investigated the capacity of preschool children aged 3-5 years (n = 28) to modify their speaking styles in response to background noise, referred to as noise-adapted speech, and when talking to an interlocutor who pretended to have hearing loss, referred to as clear speech. We examined how two modified speaking styles differed across the age range. Prosody features of conversational, noise-adapted, and clear speech were analyzed, including F0 mean (Hz), F0 range (Hz), energy in 1-3 kHz range (dB), speaking rate (syllables per second), and the number of pauses. Preschoolers adjusted their prosody features in response to auditory feedback interruptions (i.e., noise-adapted speech), while developmental changes were observed across the age range for clear speech. To examine the functional effect of the modified hyper-speech produced by the preschoolers, speech intelligibility was also examined in adult listeners (n = 30). The study found that speech intelligibility was higher in noise-adapted speech than in conversational speech across the preschool age range. A noticeable increase in speech intelligibility for clear speech was observed with the increasing age of preschool talkers, aligning with the age-related enhancements in acoustic prosody for clear speech. The findings indicate that children progressively develop their ability to modify speech in challenging environments, initiating and refining adaptations to better accommodate their listeners.

当孩子们开始在不同的交流环境中与不同的对话伙伴互动时,调整自己的说话风格尤为重要。这项研究调查了3-5岁的学龄前儿童(n = 28)在面对背景噪音时调整自己说话风格的能力,这被称为“噪音适应语言”,而当与假装听力损失的对话者交谈时,这被称为“清晰语言”。我们研究了两种经过修改的说话风格在不同年龄段的差异。分析了会话、噪声适应和清晰语音的韵律特征,包括F0平均值(Hz)、F0范围(Hz)、1-3 kHz范围内的能量(dB)、说话速率(每秒音节数)和停顿次数。学龄前儿童调整他们的韵律特征以响应听觉反馈中断(即噪音适应语音),而在整个年龄范围内观察到清晰语音的发展变化。为了检验学龄前儿童产生的改良超语言的功能效应,我们还对成年听者的语音清晰度进行了检查(n = 30)。研究发现,在整个学龄前范围内,噪音适应语音的语音清晰度高于会话语音。随着学龄前说话者年龄的增长,清晰言语的可理解性显著提高,这与清晰言语的声学韵律的年龄相关增强相一致。研究结果表明,孩子们在具有挑战性的环境中逐渐发展他们修改语言的能力,开始并改进适应,以更好地适应他们的听众。
{"title":"Prosodic Modifications to Challenging Communicative Environments in Preschoolers.","authors":"Hoyoung Yi, Delaney DiCristofaro, Woonyoung Song","doi":"10.1177/00238309241306748","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241306748","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adapting one's speaking style is particularly crucial as children start interacting with diverse conversational partners in various communication contexts. The study investigated the capacity of preschool children aged 3-5 years (<i>n</i> = 28) to modify their speaking styles in response to background noise, referred to as noise-adapted speech, and when talking to an interlocutor who pretended to have hearing loss, referred to as clear speech. We examined how two modified speaking styles differed across the age range. Prosody features of conversational, noise-adapted, and clear speech were analyzed, including F0 mean (Hz), F0 range (Hz), energy in 1-3 kHz range (dB), speaking rate (syllables per second), and the number of pauses. Preschoolers adjusted their prosody features in response to auditory feedback interruptions (i.e., noise-adapted speech), while developmental changes were observed across the age range for clear speech. To examine the functional effect of the modified hyper-speech produced by the preschoolers, speech intelligibility was also examined in adult listeners (<i>n</i> = 30). The study found that speech intelligibility was higher in noise-adapted speech than in conversational speech across the preschool age range. A noticeable increase in speech intelligibility for clear speech was observed with the increasing age of preschool talkers, aligning with the age-related enhancements in acoustic prosody for clear speech. The findings indicate that children progressively develop their ability to modify speech in challenging environments, initiating and refining adaptations to better accommodate their listeners.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241306748"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142928328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexibility and Stability in Lexical Tone Recalibration: Evidence from Tone Perceptual Learning. 词汇调校的灵活性和稳定性:来自声调知觉学习的证据。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-18 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241291536
Yingyi Luo, Holger Mitterer, Xiaolin Zhou, Yiya Chen

Listeners adjust their perception of sound categories when confronted with variations in speech. Previous research on speech recalibration has primarily focused on segmental variation, demonstrating that recalibration tends to be specific to individual speakers and situations and often persists over time. In this study, we present findings on the perceptual learning of lexical tone in Standard Chinese, a suprasegmental feature signaled primarily through pitch variations to distinguish morpheme/word meanings. Native speakers of Standard Chinese showed a recalibration of tone category boundaries immediately following exposure to ambiguous tonal pitch contours. However, this recalibration effect significantly weakened after 12 hours. Furthermore, participants trained at night did not exhibit delayed stabilization, a phenomenon commonly observed during sleep-induced consolidation. Our results replicate previous findings and provide new evidence suggesting that while our perceptual system can flexibly adapt to real-time sensory inputs, subsequent consolidation processes, such as those occurring during sleep, may exhibit selectivity and, under certain conditions, may be ineffective.

听者在面对言语变化时调整他们对声音类别的感知。先前关于语音重新校准的研究主要集中在片段变化上,表明重新校准往往是针对个别说话者和情况的,并且通常会持续一段时间。在本研究中,我们介绍了标准汉语词汇声调感知学习的研究结果,声调是一种主要通过音高变化来区分语素/词义的超分词特征。以标准汉语为母语的人在接触到模糊的声调音高轮廓后,立即对声调类别边界进行了重新校准。然而,这种重新校准效果在12小时后明显减弱。此外,在夜间训练的参与者没有表现出延迟稳定,这是在睡眠诱发巩固过程中常见的现象。我们的研究结果重复了之前的发现,并提供了新的证据,表明虽然我们的感知系统可以灵活地适应实时感官输入,但随后的巩固过程,例如在睡眠期间发生的过程,可能表现出选择性,并且在某些条件下可能无效。
{"title":"Flexibility and Stability in Lexical Tone Recalibration: Evidence from Tone Perceptual Learning.","authors":"Yingyi Luo, Holger Mitterer, Xiaolin Zhou, Yiya Chen","doi":"10.1177/00238309241291536","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241291536","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Listeners adjust their perception of sound categories when confronted with variations in speech. Previous research on speech recalibration has primarily focused on segmental variation, demonstrating that recalibration tends to be specific to individual speakers and situations and often persists over time. In this study, we present findings on the perceptual learning of lexical tone in Standard Chinese, a suprasegmental feature signaled primarily through pitch variations to distinguish morpheme/word meanings. Native speakers of Standard Chinese showed a recalibration of tone category boundaries immediately following exposure to ambiguous tonal pitch contours. However, this recalibration effect significantly weakened after 12 hours. Furthermore, participants trained at night did not exhibit delayed stabilization, a phenomenon commonly observed during sleep-induced consolidation. Our results replicate previous findings and provide new evidence suggesting that while our perceptual system can flexibly adapt to real-time sensory inputs, subsequent consolidation processes, such as those occurring during sleep, may exhibit selectivity and, under certain conditions, may be ineffective.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241291536"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142856182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptually Easy Second-Language Phones Are Not Always Easy: The Role of Orthography and Phonology in Schwa Realization in Second-Language French. 感知上容易的第二语言电话并不总是容易的:正字法和音系在第二语言法语弱读音实现中的作用。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241277995
Elisabeth Heiszenberger, Eva Reinisch, Frederik Hartmann, Elizabeth Brown, Elissa Pustka

Encoding and establishing a new second-language (L2) phonological category is notoriously difficult. This is particularly true for phonological contrasts that do not exist in the learners' native language (L1). Phonological categories that also exist in the L1 do not seem to pose any problems. However, foreign-language learners are not only presented with oral input. Instructed L2 learning often involves heavy reliance on written forms of the target language. The present study investigates the contribution of orthography to the quality of phonolexical encoding by examining the acoustics of French schwa by Austrian German learners-a perceptually and articulatorily easy L2 phone with incongruent grapheme-phoneme correspondences between the L1 and L2. We compared production patterns in an auditory word-repetition task (without orthographic input) with those in a word-reading task. We analyzed the formant values (F1, F2, F3) of the schwa realizations of two groups of Austrian high-school students who had been learning French for 1 and 6 years. The results show that production patterns are more likely to be affected by L1 grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences when orthographic input is present. However, orthography does not appear to play the dominant role, as L2 development patterns are strongly determined by both the speaker and especially the lexical item, suggesting a highly complex interaction of multiple internal and external factors in the establishment of L2 phonological categories beyond orthography and phonology.

编码和建立一个新的第二语言(L2)语音类别是出了名的困难。对于学习者的母语(L1)中不存在的语音对比尤其如此。同样存在于第一语言中的语音范畴似乎不会造成任何问题。然而,外语学习者不仅仅是口头输入。指导下的第二语言学习通常严重依赖目标语言的书面形式。本研究通过考察奥地利德语学习者的法语弱读音(一种在感知上和发音上都很容易的L2语音,L1和L2之间的字素-音素对应不一致),来调查正字法对语音编码质量的贡献。我们比较了听觉单词重复任务(没有正字法输入)和单词阅读任务中的生成模式。我们分析了两组学习法语1年和6年的奥地利高中生的弱读音实现的形成峰值(F1, F2, F3)。结果表明,当正字法输入存在时,生产模式更有可能受到L1字形与音素对应的影响。然而,正字法似乎并没有发挥主导作用,因为二语发展模式在很大程度上是由说话者,尤其是词汇项目决定的,这表明在建立二语正字法和音系之外的多种内部和外部因素的高度复杂的相互作用。
{"title":"Perceptually Easy Second-Language Phones Are Not Always Easy: The Role of Orthography and Phonology in Schwa Realization in Second-Language French.","authors":"Elisabeth Heiszenberger, Eva Reinisch, Frederik Hartmann, Elizabeth Brown, Elissa Pustka","doi":"10.1177/00238309241277995","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241277995","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Encoding and establishing a new second-language (L2) phonological category is notoriously difficult. This is particularly true for phonological contrasts that do not exist in the learners' native language (L1). Phonological categories that also exist in the L1 do not seem to pose any problems. However, foreign-language learners are not only presented with oral input. Instructed L2 learning often involves heavy reliance on written forms of the target language. The present study investigates the contribution of orthography to the quality of phonolexical encoding by examining the acoustics of French schwa by Austrian German learners-a perceptually and articulatorily easy L2 phone with incongruent grapheme-phoneme correspondences between the L1 and L2. We compared production patterns in an auditory word-repetition task (without orthographic input) with those in a word-reading task. We analyzed the formant values (F1, F2, F3) of the schwa realizations of two groups of Austrian high-school students who had been learning French for 1 and 6 years. The results show that production patterns are more likely to be affected by L1 grapheme-to-phoneme correspondences when orthographic input is present. However, orthography does not appear to play the dominant role, as L2 development patterns are strongly determined by both the speaker and especially the lexical item, suggesting a highly complex interaction of multiple internal and external factors in the establishment of L2 phonological categories beyond orthography and phonology.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241277995"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142815006","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Child Consonant Harmony Revisited: The Role of Lexical Memory Constraints and Segment Repetition. 儿童辅音和谐:词汇记忆限制与词段重复的作用。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-03 DOI: 10.1177/00238309241297703
Mitsuhiko Ota

Young children often produce non-target-like word forms in which non-adjacent consonants share a major place of articulation (e.g., [gɔgi] "doggy"). Termed child consonant harmony (CCH), this phenomenon has garnered considerable attention in the literature, primarily due to the apparent absence of analogous patterns in mature phonological systems. This study takes a close look at a potential account of CCH that is compatible with findings from adult word learning, serial recall, and phonological typology. According to this account, CCH is a response to memory pressure involved in remembering and retrieving multiple consonantal contrasts within a word. If this is the main motivation behind CCH, we would expect the resulting child forms to be biased toward full assimilation (i.e., consonant repetition) as it allows maximal reduction of phonolexical memory load. To test this prediction, children's productions of target words containing consonants that differ in both major place and manner were analyzed using two data sources: a single session sample from 40 children aged 1-2 years learning English, French, Finnish, Japanese, or Mandarin; and longitudinal samples from seven English-learning children between 1 and 3 years of age. Prevalence of consonant repetitions was robustly evidenced in early child forms, especially in those produced for target words with the structure CVCV(C). The results suggest that early word production is shaped by constraints on phonolexical memory.

年幼的孩子经常会说出非目标式的单词形式,其中非相邻的辅音共享一个主要的发音位置(例如,[g] gi]“doggy”)。这种现象被称为儿童辅音和谐(CCH),在文献中引起了相当大的关注,主要是因为在成熟的语音系统中明显缺乏类似的模式。这项研究密切关注了CCH的潜在解释,这与成人单词学习、序列回忆和语音类型学的发现是一致的。根据这种说法,CCH是对记忆压力的一种反应,涉及记忆和检索单词中的多个辅音对比。如果这是CCH背后的主要动机,我们可以预期,由此产生的儿童形式倾向于完全同化(即辅音重复),因为它可以最大限度地减少语音记忆负荷。为了验证这一预测,我们使用两个数据来源分析了含有不同主要位置和方式的辅音的儿童产生的目标单词:一个来自40名1-2岁学习英语、法语、芬兰语、日语或普通话的儿童的单次会话样本;以及7名1至3岁英语学习儿童的纵向样本。在儿童早期形式中,辅音重复的流行得到了有力的证明,特别是在那些为CVCV(C)结构的目标单词产生的单词中。结果表明,早期的单词生成是由语音记忆的限制所决定的。
{"title":"Child Consonant Harmony Revisited: The Role of Lexical Memory Constraints and Segment Repetition.","authors":"Mitsuhiko Ota","doi":"10.1177/00238309241297703","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309241297703","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Young children often produce non-target-like word forms in which non-adjacent consonants share a major place of articulation (e.g., [gɔgi] \"doggy\"). Termed child consonant harmony (CCH), this phenomenon has garnered considerable attention in the literature, primarily due to the apparent absence of analogous patterns in mature phonological systems. This study takes a close look at a potential account of CCH that is compatible with findings from adult word learning, serial recall, and phonological typology. According to this account, CCH is a response to memory pressure involved in remembering and retrieving multiple consonantal contrasts within a word. If this is the main motivation behind CCH, we would expect the resulting child forms to be biased toward full assimilation (i.e., consonant repetition) as it allows maximal reduction of phonolexical memory load. To test this prediction, children's productions of target words containing consonants that differ in both major place and manner were analyzed using two data sources: a single session sample from 40 children aged 1-2 years learning English, French, Finnish, Japanese, or Mandarin; and longitudinal samples from seven English-learning children between 1 and 3 years of age. Prevalence of consonant repetitions was robustly evidenced in early child forms, especially in those produced for target words with the structure CVCV(C). The results suggest that early word production is shaped by constraints on phonolexical memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"238309241297703"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142774382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contrastive Alveolar/Retroflex Phonemes in Singapore Mandarin Bilinguals: Comprehension Rates for Articulations in Different Accents, and Acoustic Analysis of Productions. 新加坡普通话双语者的牙槽音/后弯音对比:不同重音发音的理解率和产生的声学分析。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 Q3 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1177/00238309231205012
Hannah L Goh, Fei Ting Woon, Scott R Moisik, Suzy J Styles

The standard Beijing variety of Mandarin has a clear alveolar-retroflex contrast for phonemes featuring voiceless sibilant frication (i.e., /s/, /ʂ/, /ʈs/, /ʈʂ/, /ʈsʰ/, /ʈʂʰ/). However, some studies show that varieties in the 'outer circle', such in Taiwan, have a reduced contrast for these speech sounds via a process known as 'deretroflexion'. The variety of Mandarin spoken in Singapore is also considered as 'outer circle', as it exhibits influences from Min Nan varieties. We investigated how bilinguals of Singapore Mandarin and English perceive and produce speech tokens in minimal pairs differing only in the alveolar/retroflex place of articulation. In all, 50 participants took part in two tasks. In Task 1, participants performed a lexical identification task for minimal pairs differing only the alveolar/retroflex place of articulation, as spoken by native speakers of two varieties: Beijing Mandarin and Singapore Mandarin. No difference in comprehension of the words was observed between the two varieties indicating that both varieties contain sufficient acoustic information for discrimination. In Task 2, participants read aloud from the list of minimal pairs while their voices were recorded. Acoustic analysis revealed that the phonemes do indeed differ acoustically in terms of center of gravity of the frication and in an alternative measure: long-term averaged spectra. The magnitude of this difference appears to be smaller than previously reported differences for the Beijing variety. These findings show that although some deretroflexion is evident in the speech of bilinguals of the Singaporean variety of Mandarin, it does not translate to ambiguity in the speech signal.

标准的北京普通话变体具有明显的肺泡后屈对比,其音素以无声嘶擦音为特征(即/s/,/ʂ/,/ 648; s/,/ʈsʰ/、/648ʂ;/)。然而,一些研究表明,“外圈”的变体,如台湾的变体,通过一个被称为“去屈折”的过程,这些语音的对比度降低。新加坡普通话的变体也被认为是“外环”,因为它受到闽南语变体的影响。我们调查了新加坡普通话和英语的双语者如何感知和产生仅在牙槽/后弯发音位置不同的最小配对的语音标记。总共有50名参与者参加了两项任务。在任务1中,参与者对只有牙槽/后屈发音位置不同的最小配对进行了词汇识别任务,这是两种母语为北京普通话和新加坡普通话的人所说的。两个变体对单词的理解没有差异,这表明两个变体都包含足够的声学信息进行区分。在任务2中,参与者在录音的同时大声朗读最小配对的列表。声学分析表明,就摩擦的重心和另一种衡量标准:长期平均频谱而言,这些音位在声学上确实存在差异。这种差异的幅度似乎小于之前报道的北京品种的差异。这些发现表明,尽管新加坡普通话双语者的言语中存在明显的去屈折现象,但它不会转化为言语信号中的歧义。
{"title":"Contrastive Alveolar/Retroflex Phonemes in Singapore Mandarin Bilinguals: Comprehension Rates for Articulations in Different Accents, and Acoustic Analysis of Productions.","authors":"Hannah L Goh, Fei Ting Woon, Scott R Moisik, Suzy J Styles","doi":"10.1177/00238309231205012","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00238309231205012","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The standard Beijing variety of Mandarin has a clear alveolar-retroflex contrast for phonemes featuring voiceless sibilant frication (i.e., /s/, /ʂ/, /ʈs/, /ʈʂ/, /ʈsʰ/, /ʈʂʰ/). However, some studies show that varieties in the 'outer circle', such in Taiwan, have a reduced contrast for these speech sounds via a process known as 'deretroflexion'. The variety of Mandarin spoken in Singapore is also considered as 'outer circle', as it exhibits influences from Min Nan varieties. We investigated how bilinguals of Singapore Mandarin and English perceive and produce speech tokens in minimal pairs differing only in the alveolar/retroflex place of articulation. In all, 50 participants took part in two tasks. In Task 1, participants performed a lexical identification task for minimal pairs differing only the alveolar/retroflex place of articulation, as spoken by native speakers of two varieties: Beijing Mandarin and Singapore Mandarin. No difference in comprehension of the words was observed between the two varieties indicating that both varieties contain sufficient acoustic information for discrimination. In Task 2, participants read aloud from the list of minimal pairs while their voices were recorded. Acoustic analysis revealed that the phonemes do indeed differ acoustically in terms of center of gravity of the frication and in an alternative measure: long-term averaged spectra. The magnitude of this difference appears to be smaller than previously reported differences for the Beijing variety. These findings show that although some deretroflexion is evident in the speech of bilinguals of the Singaporean variety of Mandarin, it does not translate to ambiguity in the speech signal.</p>","PeriodicalId":51255,"journal":{"name":"Language and Speech","volume":" ","pages":"924-944"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72016107","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Language and Speech
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1