首页 > 最新文献

Multisensory Research最新文献

英文 中文
Audiovisual Speech Perception Benefits are Stable from Preschool through Adolescence. 视听言语感知的益处从学龄前到青春期都很稳定。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10128
Liesbeth Gijbels, Jason D Yeatman, Kaylah Lalonde, Piper Doering, Adrian K C Lee

The ability to leverage visual cues in speech perception - especially in noisy backgrounds - is well established from infancy to adulthood. Yet, the developmental trajectory of audiovisual benefits stays a topic of debate. The inconsistency in findings can be attributed to relatively small sample sizes or tasks that are not appropriate for given age groups. We designed an audiovisual speech perception task that was cognitively and linguistically age-appropriate from preschool to adolescence and recruited a large sample ( N = 161) of children (age 4-15). We found that even the youngest children show reliable speech perception benefits when provided with visual cues and that these benefits are consistent throughout development when auditory and visual signals match. Individual variability is explained by how the child experiences their speech-in-noise performance rather than the quality of the signal itself. This underscores the importance of visual speech for young children who are regularly in noisy environments like classrooms and playgrounds.

从婴儿期到成年期,利用视觉线索进行语音感知(尤其是在嘈杂的背景下)的能力已得到广泛认可。然而,视听益处的发展轨迹仍是一个争论不休的话题。研究结果不一致的原因可能是样本量相对较小或任务不适合特定年龄组。我们设计了一项视听言语感知任务,该任务在认知和语言方面适合学龄前到青少年的年龄,并招募了大量儿童样本(N = 161)(4-15 岁)。我们发现,即使是最年幼的儿童,在获得视觉线索时也会表现出可靠的语音感知优势,而且当听觉和视觉信号相匹配时,这些优势在整个发育过程中都是一致的。个体差异的原因在于儿童如何体验他们的噪声语音表现,而不是信号本身的质量。这强调了视觉语言对经常处于嘈杂环境(如教室和操场)中的幼儿的重要性。
{"title":"Audiovisual Speech Perception Benefits are Stable from Preschool through Adolescence.","authors":"Liesbeth Gijbels, Jason D Yeatman, Kaylah Lalonde, Piper Doering, Adrian K C Lee","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10128","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10128","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The ability to leverage visual cues in speech perception - especially in noisy backgrounds - is well established from infancy to adulthood. Yet, the developmental trajectory of audiovisual benefits stays a topic of debate. The inconsistency in findings can be attributed to relatively small sample sizes or tasks that are not appropriate for given age groups. We designed an audiovisual speech perception task that was cognitively and linguistically age-appropriate from preschool to adolescence and recruited a large sample ( N = 161) of children (age 4-15). We found that even the youngest children show reliable speech perception benefits when provided with visual cues and that these benefits are consistent throughout development when auditory and visual signals match. Individual variability is explained by how the child experiences their speech-in-noise performance rather than the quality of the signal itself. This underscores the importance of visual speech for young children who are regularly in noisy environments like classrooms and playgrounds.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"317-340"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Multisensory Olfactory Training Improve Olfactory Dysfunction Caused by COVID-19? 多感官嗅觉训练能改善 COVID-19 引起的嗅觉功能障碍吗?
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10127
Gözde Filiz, Simon Bérubé, Claudia Demers, Frank Cloutier, Angela Chen, Valérie Pek, Émilie Hudon, Josiane Bolduc-Bégin, Johannes Frasnelli

Approximately 30-60% of people suffer from olfactory dysfunction (OD) such as hyposmia or anosmia after being diagnosed with COVID-19; 15-20% of these cases last beyond resolution of the acute phase. Previous studies have shown that olfactory training can be beneficial for patients affected by OD caused by viral infections of the upper respiratory tract. The aim of the study is to evaluate whether a multisensory olfactory training involving simultaneously tasting and seeing congruent stimuli is more effective than the classical olfactory training. We recruited 68 participants with persistent OD for two months or more after COVID-19 infection; they were divided into three groups. One group received olfactory training which involved smelling four odorants (strawberry, cheese, coffee, lemon; classical olfactory training). The other group received the same olfactory stimuli but presented retronasally (i.e., as droplets on their tongue); while simultaneous and congruent gustatory (i.e., sweet, salty, bitter, sour) and visual (corresponding images) stimuli were presented (multisensory olfactory training). The third group received odorless propylene glycol in four bottles (control group). Training was carried out twice daily for 12 weeks. We assessed olfactory function and olfactory specific quality of life before and after the intervention. Both intervention groups showed a similar significant improvement of olfactory function, although there was no difference in the assessment of quality of life. Both multisensory and classical training can be beneficial for OD following a viral infection; however, only the classical olfactory training paradigm leads to an improvement that was significantly stronger than the control group.

约有 30-60% 的人在确诊感染 COVID-19 后会出现嗅觉障碍(OD),如嗅觉减退或嗅觉缺失;其中 15-20% 的病例会持续到急性期过后。以往的研究表明,嗅觉训练对上呼吸道病毒感染引起的嗅觉障碍患者有益。本研究旨在评估同时品尝和观看一致刺激物的多感官嗅觉训练是否比传统的嗅觉训练更有效。我们招募了 68 名感染 COVID-19 后持续两个月或更长时间的 OD 患者,将他们分为三组。一组接受嗅觉训练,包括闻四种气味(草莓、奶酪、咖啡、柠檬;经典嗅觉训练)。另一组接受相同的嗅觉刺激,但以舌尖上的液滴形式出现(多感官嗅觉训练);同时出现的还有一致的味觉(即甜、咸、苦、酸)和视觉(相应的图像)刺激。第三组接受四瓶无味丙二醇(对照组)。训练每天进行两次,持续 12 周。我们对干预前后的嗅觉功能和嗅觉特定生活质量进行了评估。尽管在生活质量评估方面没有差异,但两个干预组的嗅觉功能都有类似的明显改善。多感官训练和经典训练对病毒感染后的嗅觉障碍都有好处;但是,只有经典嗅觉训练范式的改善效果明显强于对照组。
{"title":"Can Multisensory Olfactory Training Improve Olfactory Dysfunction Caused by COVID-19?","authors":"Gözde Filiz, Simon Bérubé, Claudia Demers, Frank Cloutier, Angela Chen, Valérie Pek, Émilie Hudon, Josiane Bolduc-Bégin, Johannes Frasnelli","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10127","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10127","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Approximately 30-60% of people suffer from olfactory dysfunction (OD) such as hyposmia or anosmia after being diagnosed with COVID-19; 15-20% of these cases last beyond resolution of the acute phase. Previous studies have shown that olfactory training can be beneficial for patients affected by OD caused by viral infections of the upper respiratory tract. The aim of the study is to evaluate whether a multisensory olfactory training involving simultaneously tasting and seeing congruent stimuli is more effective than the classical olfactory training. We recruited 68 participants with persistent OD for two months or more after COVID-19 infection; they were divided into three groups. One group received olfactory training which involved smelling four odorants (strawberry, cheese, coffee, lemon; classical olfactory training). The other group received the same olfactory stimuli but presented retronasally (i.e., as droplets on their tongue); while simultaneous and congruent gustatory (i.e., sweet, salty, bitter, sour) and visual (corresponding images) stimuli were presented (multisensory olfactory training). The third group received odorless propylene glycol in four bottles (control group). Training was carried out twice daily for 12 weeks. We assessed olfactory function and olfactory specific quality of life before and after the intervention. Both intervention groups showed a similar significant improvement of olfactory function, although there was no difference in the assessment of quality of life. Both multisensory and classical training can be beneficial for OD following a viral infection; however, only the classical olfactory training paradigm leads to an improvement that was significantly stronger than the control group.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"299-316"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Glassware Influences the Perception of Orange Juice in Simulated Naturalistic versus Urban Conditions. 玻璃器皿在模拟自然环境与城市环境下对橙汁感知的影响
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10126
Chunmao Wu, Pei Li, Charles Spence

The latest research demonstrates that people's perception of orange juice can be influenced by the shape/type of receptacle in which it happens to be served. Two studies are reported that were designed to investigate the impact, if any, that the shape/type of glass might exert over the perception of the contents, the emotions induced on tasting the juice and the consumer's intention to purchase orange juice. The same quantity of orange juice (100 ml) was presented and evaluated in three different glasses: a straight-sided, a curved and a tapered glass. Questionnaires were used to assess taste (aroma, flavour intensity, sweetness, freshness and fruitiness), pleasantness and intention to buy orange juice. Study 2 assessed the impact of the same three glasses in two digitally rendered atmospheric conditions (nature vs urban). In Study 1, the perceived sweetness and pleasantness of the orange juice was significantly influenced by the shape/type of the glass in which it was presented. Study 2 reported significant interactions between condition (nature vs urban) and glass shape (tapered, straight-sided and curved). Perceived aroma, flavour intensity and pleasantness were all significantly affected by the simulated audiovisual context or atmosphere. Compared to the urban condition, perceived aroma, freshness, fruitiness and pleasantness were rated significantly higher in the nature condition. On the other hand, flavour intensity and sweetness were rated significantly higher in the urban condition than in the natural condition. These results are likely to be relevant for those interested in providing food services, or company managers offering beverages to their customers.

最新研究表明,人们对橙汁的感知会受到盛放橙汁的容器形状/类型的影响。报告中的两项研究旨在调查玻璃杯的形状/类型是否会影响人们对橙汁内容物的感知、品尝橙汁时产生的情绪以及消费者购买橙汁的意愿。同样数量的橙汁(100 毫升)分别装在三种不同的玻璃杯中进行品评:直杯、弯杯和锥形杯。采用问卷调查的方式来评估口味(香气、味道强度、甜度、新鲜度和果味)、愉悦度和购买橙汁的意愿。研究 2 评估了同样的三种玻璃杯在两种数字渲染的大气环境(自然环境和城市环境)下的影响。在研究 1 中,橙汁的甜度和愉悦度受玻璃杯形状/类型的显著影响。研究 2 显示,条件(自然环境与城市环境)和玻璃杯形状(锥形、直角形和弧形)之间存在明显的交互作用。所感受到的香气、味道强度和愉悦度都受到模拟视听环境或氛围的显著影响。与城市条件相比,在自然条件下,人们对香气、新鲜度、果味和愉悦度的评价明显更高。另一方面,味道强度和甜度在城市条件下的评分明显高于自然条件下。这些结果可能对有意提供食品服务的人员或向客户提供饮料的公司经理有借鉴意义。
{"title":"Glassware Influences the Perception of Orange Juice in Simulated Naturalistic versus Urban Conditions.","authors":"Chunmao Wu, Pei Li, Charles Spence","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10126","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10126","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The latest research demonstrates that people's perception of orange juice can be influenced by the shape/type of receptacle in which it happens to be served. Two studies are reported that were designed to investigate the impact, if any, that the shape/type of glass might exert over the perception of the contents, the emotions induced on tasting the juice and the consumer's intention to purchase orange juice. The same quantity of orange juice (100 ml) was presented and evaluated in three different glasses: a straight-sided, a curved and a tapered glass. Questionnaires were used to assess taste (aroma, flavour intensity, sweetness, freshness and fruitiness), pleasantness and intention to buy orange juice. Study 2 assessed the impact of the same three glasses in two digitally rendered atmospheric conditions (nature vs urban). In Study 1, the perceived sweetness and pleasantness of the orange juice was significantly influenced by the shape/type of the glass in which it was presented. Study 2 reported significant interactions between condition (nature vs urban) and glass shape (tapered, straight-sided and curved). Perceived aroma, flavour intensity and pleasantness were all significantly affected by the simulated audiovisual context or atmosphere. Compared to the urban condition, perceived aroma, freshness, fruitiness and pleasantness were rated significantly higher in the nature condition. On the other hand, flavour intensity and sweetness were rated significantly higher in the urban condition than in the natural condition. These results are likely to be relevant for those interested in providing food services, or company managers offering beverages to their customers.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"275-297"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual Adaptation to Noise-Vocoded Speech by Lip-Read Information: No Difference between Dyslexic and Typical Readers. 通过唇读信息对噪声编码语音的感知适应:阅读障碍者与典型阅读者之间没有差异。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10125
Faezeh Pourhashemi, Martijn Baart, Jean Vroomen

Auditory speech can be difficult to understand but seeing the articulatory movements of a speaker can drastically improve spoken-word recognition and, on the longer-term, it helps listeners to adapt to acoustically distorted speech. Given that individuals with developmental dyslexia (DD) have sometimes been reported to rely less on lip-read speech than typical readers, we examined lip-read-driven adaptation to distorted speech in a group of adults with DD ( N = 29) and a comparison group of typical readers ( N = 29). Participants were presented with acoustically distorted Dutch words (six-channel noise-vocoded speech, NVS) in audiovisual training blocks (where the speaker could be seen) interspersed with audio-only test blocks. Results showed that words were more accurately recognized if the speaker could be seen (a lip-read advantage), and that performance steadily improved across subsequent auditory-only test blocks (adaptation). There were no group differences, suggesting that perceptual adaptation to disrupted spoken words is comparable for dyslexic and typical readers. These data open up a research avenue to investigate the degree to which lip-read-driven speech adaptation generalizes across different types of auditory degradation, and across dyslexic readers with decoding versus comprehension difficulties.

听觉语言可能难以理解,但看到说话者的发音动作可以大大提高口语单词的识别能力,而且从长远来看,还有助于听者适应声音失真的语音。据报道,发育性阅读障碍(DD)患者对唇读语音的依赖程度有时低于典型读者,有鉴于此,我们对一组发育性阅读障碍成人(29 人)和一组典型读者(29 人)进行了唇读驱动的失真语音适应性测试。在视听训练区块(可以看到说话者)和纯音频测试区块中,向参与者展示了声音失真的荷兰语单词(六通道噪声编码语音,NVS)。结果表明,如果能看到说话者,就能更准确地识别单词(唇读优势),而且在随后的纯听觉测试块中,成绩稳步提高(适应)。结果显示,读写障碍者和典型阅读障碍者对中断的口语单词的感知适应能力相当。这些数据为研究唇读驱动的语音适应性在不同类型的听觉退化中的普遍程度,以及在有解码和理解困难的阅读障碍读者中的普遍程度开辟了一条研究途径。
{"title":"Perceptual Adaptation to Noise-Vocoded Speech by Lip-Read Information: No Difference between Dyslexic and Typical Readers.","authors":"Faezeh Pourhashemi, Martijn Baart, Jean Vroomen","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10125","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10125","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Auditory speech can be difficult to understand but seeing the articulatory movements of a speaker can drastically improve spoken-word recognition and, on the longer-term, it helps listeners to adapt to acoustically distorted speech. Given that individuals with developmental dyslexia (DD) have sometimes been reported to rely less on lip-read speech than typical readers, we examined lip-read-driven adaptation to distorted speech in a group of adults with DD ( N = 29) and a comparison group of typical readers ( N = 29). Participants were presented with acoustically distorted Dutch words (six-channel noise-vocoded speech, NVS) in audiovisual training blocks (where the speaker could be seen) interspersed with audio-only test blocks. Results showed that words were more accurately recognized if the speaker could be seen (a lip-read advantage), and that performance steadily improved across subsequent auditory-only test blocks (adaptation). There were no group differences, suggesting that perceptual adaptation to disrupted spoken words is comparable for dyslexic and typical readers. These data open up a research avenue to investigate the degree to which lip-read-driven speech adaptation generalizes across different types of auditory degradation, and across dyslexic readers with decoding versus comprehension difficulties.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"243-259"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is Front associated with Above and Back with Below? Association between Allocentric Representations of Spatial Dimensions. 前与上相关,后与下相关吗?空间维度所有中心表征之间的关联。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10124
Lari Vainio, Martti Vainio

Previous research has revealed congruency effects between different spatial dimensions such as right and up. In the audiovisual context, high-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of up/above and front, while low-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of down/below and back. This opens the question of whether there could also be a spatial association between above and front and/or below and back. Participants were presented with a high- or low-pitch stimulus at the time of the onset of the visual stimulus. In one block, participants responded according to the above/below location of the visual target stimulus if the target appeared in front of the reference object, and in the other block, they performed these above/below responses if the target appeared at the back of the reference. In general, reaction times revealed an advantage in processing the target location in the front-above and back-below locations. The front-above/back-below effect was more robust concerning the back-below component of the effect, and significantly larger in reaction times that were slower rather than faster than the median value of a participant. However, the pitch did not robustly influence responding to front/back or above/below locations. We propose that this effect might be based on the conceptual association between different spatial dimensions.

以往的研究揭示了不同空间维度(如右和上)之间的一致性效应。在视听语境中,高音调的声音与上/上方和前方的空间维度相关联,而低音调的声音则与下/下方和后方的空间维度相关联。这就提出了一个问题,即上方和前方和/或下方和后方之间是否也存在空间关联。在视觉刺激开始时,参与者会看到高音调或低音调的刺激。在一个区块中,如果视觉目标刺激出现在参照物的前面,受试者就会根据视觉目标刺激的上方/下方位置做出反应;而在另一个区块中,如果视觉目标刺激出现在参照物的后面,受试者就会根据视觉目标刺激的上方/下方位置做出反应。总的来说,反应时间表明,在前上方和后下方位置处理目标位置具有优势。前上方/后下方效应与后下方效应相比更为稳健,在反应时间慢于而不是快于被试的中位值时,前上方/后下方效应明显更大。然而,音调对前后或上下位置的反应并无显著影响。我们认为,这种效应可能是基于不同空间维度之间的概念关联。
{"title":"Is Front associated with Above and Back with Below? Association between Allocentric Representations of Spatial Dimensions.","authors":"Lari Vainio, Martti Vainio","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10124","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10124","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has revealed congruency effects between different spatial dimensions such as right and up. In the audiovisual context, high-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of up/above and front, while low-pitched sounds are associated with the spatial dimensions of down/below and back. This opens the question of whether there could also be a spatial association between above and front and/or below and back. Participants were presented with a high- or low-pitch stimulus at the time of the onset of the visual stimulus. In one block, participants responded according to the above/below location of the visual target stimulus if the target appeared in front of the reference object, and in the other block, they performed these above/below responses if the target appeared at the back of the reference. In general, reaction times revealed an advantage in processing the target location in the front-above and back-below locations. The front-above/back-below effect was more robust concerning the back-below component of the effect, and significantly larger in reaction times that were slower rather than faster than the median value of a participant. However, the pitch did not robustly influence responding to front/back or above/below locations. We propose that this effect might be based on the conceptual association between different spatial dimensions.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"217-241"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the Deviation Effects of Irrelevant Sound on Serial and Nonserial Tasks. 重新审视连贯任务和非连贯任务中无关声音的偏差效应
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10123
Yu Nakajima, Hiroshi Ashida

Two types of disruptive effects of irrelevant sound on visual tasks have been reported: the changing-state effect and the deviation effect. The idea that the deviation effect, which arises from attentional capture, is independent of task requirements, whereas the changing-state effect is specific to tasks that require serial processing, has been examined by comparing tasks that do or do not require serial-order processing. While many previous studies used the missing-item task as the nonserial task, it is unclear whether other cognitive tasks lead to similar results regarding the different task specificity of both effects. Kattner et al. (Memory and Cognition, 2023) used the mental-arithmetic task as the nonserial task, and failed to demonstrate the deviation effect. However, there were several procedural factors that could account for the lack of deviation effect, such as differences in design and procedures (e.g., conducted online, intermixed conditions). In the present study, we aimed to investigate whether the deviation effect could be observed in both the serial-recall and mental-arithmetic tasks when these procedural factors were modified. We found strong evidence of the deviation effect in both the serial-recall and the mental-arithmetic tasks when stimulus presentation and experimental design were aligned with previous studies that demonstrated the deviation effect (e.g., conducted in-person, blockwise presentation of sound, etc.). The results support the idea that the deviation effect is not task-specific.

据报道,无关声音对视觉任务有两种干扰效应:变化状态效应和偏离效应。偏离效应是由注意力捕捉引起的,与任务要求无关,而变化状态效应则是需要序列处理的任务所特有的。虽然之前的许多研究都使用遗漏项任务作为非序列任务,但目前还不清楚其他认知任务是否也会导致类似的结果,即两种效应的任务特异性不同。Kattner 等人(《记忆与认知》,2023 年)使用心算任务作为非序列任务,但未能证明偏差效应。然而,有几个程序性因素可以解释偏差效应的缺乏,如设计和程序的差异(如在线进行、混合条件)。在本研究中,我们旨在探讨当这些程序性因素发生改变时,在序列回忆任务和心算任务中是否都能观察到偏差效应。我们发现,当刺激呈现和实验设计与之前证明偏差效应的研究(如当面进行、声音的顺时针呈现等)一致时,偏差效应在序列回忆和心算任务中都得到了有力的证明。这些结果支持了偏差效应并非针对特定任务的观点。
{"title":"Revisiting the Deviation Effects of Irrelevant Sound on Serial and Nonserial Tasks.","authors":"Yu Nakajima, Hiroshi Ashida","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10123","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two types of disruptive effects of irrelevant sound on visual tasks have been reported: the changing-state effect and the deviation effect. The idea that the deviation effect, which arises from attentional capture, is independent of task requirements, whereas the changing-state effect is specific to tasks that require serial processing, has been examined by comparing tasks that do or do not require serial-order processing. While many previous studies used the missing-item task as the nonserial task, it is unclear whether other cognitive tasks lead to similar results regarding the different task specificity of both effects. Kattner et al. (Memory and Cognition, 2023) used the mental-arithmetic task as the nonserial task, and failed to demonstrate the deviation effect. However, there were several procedural factors that could account for the lack of deviation effect, such as differences in design and procedures (e.g., conducted online, intermixed conditions). In the present study, we aimed to investigate whether the deviation effect could be observed in both the serial-recall and mental-arithmetic tasks when these procedural factors were modified. We found strong evidence of the deviation effect in both the serial-recall and the mental-arithmetic tasks when stimulus presentation and experimental design were aligned with previous studies that demonstrated the deviation effect (e.g., conducted in-person, blockwise presentation of sound, etc.). The results support the idea that the deviation effect is not task-specific.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"261-273"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140900337","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived Audio-Visual Simultaneity Is Recalibrated by the Visual Intensity of the Preceding Trial. 感知到的视听同时性会被前一个试验的视觉强度重新校准。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2024-04-30 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10121
Ryan Horsfall, Neil Harrison, Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger

A vital heuristic used when making judgements on whether audio-visual signals arise from the same event, is the temporal coincidence of the respective signals. Previous research has highlighted a process, whereby the perception of simultaneity rapidly recalibrates to account for differences in the physical temporal offsets of stimuli. The current paper investigated whether rapid recalibration also occurs in response to differences in central arrival latencies, driven by visual-intensity-dependent processing times. In a behavioural experiment, observers completed a temporal-order judgement (TOJ), simultaneity judgement (SJ) and simple reaction-time (RT) task and responded to audio-visual trials that were preceded by other audio-visual trials with either a bright or dim visual stimulus. It was found that the point of subjective simultaneity shifted, due to the visual intensity of the preceding stimulus, in the TOJ, but not SJ task, while the RT data revealed no effect of preceding intensity. Our data therefore provide some evidence that the perception of simultaneity rapidly recalibrates based on stimulus intensity.

在判断视听信号是否来自同一事件时,一个重要的启发式方法就是各信号的时间重合性。以往的研究强调了一个过程,即同时性感知会迅速重新校准,以考虑刺激物物理时间偏移的差异。本文研究了快速重新校准是否也会因中心到达潜伏期的不同而发生,而中心到达潜伏期是由视觉强度相关的处理时间驱动的。在一项行为实验中,观察者完成了时序判断(TOJ)、同时性判断(SJ)和简单反应时间(RT)任务,并对前面带有明亮或昏暗视觉刺激的其他视听试验做出了反应。结果发现,在 TOJ 任务中,主观同时性点会因前面刺激的视觉强度而移动,但在 SJ 任务中不会,而在 RT 数据中,前面刺激的强度没有影响。因此,我们的数据提供了一些证据,证明同时性感知会根据刺激强度迅速重新校准。
{"title":"Perceived Audio-Visual Simultaneity Is Recalibrated by the Visual Intensity of the Preceding Trial.","authors":"Ryan Horsfall, Neil Harrison, Georg Meyer, Sophie Wuerger","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10121","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-bja10121","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A vital heuristic used when making judgements on whether audio-visual signals arise from the same event, is the temporal coincidence of the respective signals. Previous research has highlighted a process, whereby the perception of simultaneity rapidly recalibrates to account for differences in the physical temporal offsets of stimuli. The current paper investigated whether rapid recalibration also occurs in response to differences in central arrival latencies, driven by visual-intensity-dependent processing times. In a behavioural experiment, observers completed a temporal-order judgement (TOJ), simultaneity judgement (SJ) and simple reaction-time (RT) task and responded to audio-visual trials that were preceded by other audio-visual trials with either a bright or dim visual stimulus. It was found that the point of subjective simultaneity shifted, due to the visual intensity of the preceding stimulus, in the TOJ, but not SJ task, while the RT data revealed no effect of preceding intensity. Our data therefore provide some evidence that the perception of simultaneity rapidly recalibrates based on stimulus intensity.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":"37 2","pages":"143-162"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140877920","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spatial Sensory References for Vestibular Self-Motion Perception. 前庭自我运动感知的空间感觉参考。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2023-12-20 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10117
Silvia Zanchi, Luigi F Cuturi, Giulio Sandini, Monica Gori, Elisa R Ferrè

While navigating through the surroundings, we constantly rely on inertial vestibular signals for self-motion along with visual and acoustic spatial references from the environment. However, the interaction between inertial cues and environmental spatial references is not yet fully understood. Here we investigated whether vestibular self-motion sensitivity is influenced by sensory spatial references. Healthy participants were administered a Vestibular Self-Motion Detection Task in which they were asked to detect vestibular self-motion sensations induced by low-intensity Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation. Participants performed this detection task with or without an external visual or acoustic spatial reference placed directly in front of them. We computed the d prime ( d ' ) as a measure of participants' vestibular sensitivity and the criterion as an index of their response bias. Results showed that the visual spatial reference increased sensitivity to detect vestibular self-motion. Conversely, the acoustic spatial reference did not influence self-motion sensitivity. Both visual and auditory spatial references did not cause changes in response bias. Environmental visual spatial references provide relevant information to enhance our ability to perceive inertial self-motion cues, suggesting a specific interaction between visual and vestibular systems in self-motion perception.

在周围环境中导航时,我们不断依靠惯性前庭信号以及来自环境的视觉和听觉空间参考来进行自我运动。然而,惯性线索与环境空间参考之间的相互作用尚未完全明了。在此,我们研究了前庭自我运动灵敏度是否受感官空间参考的影响。我们对健康的参与者进行了前庭自我运动检测任务,要求他们检测由低强度 Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation 引起的前庭自我运动感觉。受试者在有或没有外部视觉或听觉空间参照物的情况下完成这项检测任务。我们计算了 d prime ( d '),以此来衡量参与者的前庭敏感度,并计算了标准值,以此来衡量参与者的反应偏差。结果显示,视觉空间参照物提高了检测前庭自我运动的灵敏度。相反,听觉空间参照物并不影响自我运动灵敏度。视觉和听觉空间参照物都不会引起反应偏差的变化。环境视觉空间参照物提供了相关信息,提高了我们感知惯性自我运动线索的能力,这表明视觉和前庭系统在自我运动感知中存在特定的相互作用。
{"title":"Spatial Sensory References for Vestibular Self-Motion Perception.","authors":"Silvia Zanchi, Luigi F Cuturi, Giulio Sandini, Monica Gori, Elisa R Ferrè","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10117","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10117","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While navigating through the surroundings, we constantly rely on inertial vestibular signals for self-motion along with visual and acoustic spatial references from the environment. However, the interaction between inertial cues and environmental spatial references is not yet fully understood. Here we investigated whether vestibular self-motion sensitivity is influenced by sensory spatial references. Healthy participants were administered a Vestibular Self-Motion Detection Task in which they were asked to detect vestibular self-motion sensations induced by low-intensity Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation. Participants performed this detection task with or without an external visual or acoustic spatial reference placed directly in front of them. We computed the d prime ( d ' ) as a measure of participants' vestibular sensitivity and the criterion as an index of their response bias. Results showed that the visual spatial reference increased sensitivity to detect vestibular self-motion. Conversely, the acoustic spatial reference did not influence self-motion sensitivity. Both visual and auditory spatial references did not cause changes in response bias. Environmental visual spatial references provide relevant information to enhance our ability to perceive inertial self-motion cues, suggesting a specific interaction between visual and vestibular systems in self-motion perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"75-88"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-Modal Contributions to Episodic Memory for Voices. 对声音外显记忆的跨模态贡献
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2023-12-20 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10116
Joshua R Tatz, Zehra F Peynircioğlu

Multisensory context often facilitates perception and memory. In fact, encoding items within a multisensory context can improve memory even on strictly unisensory tests (i.e., when the multisensory context is absent). Prior studies that have consistently found these multisensory facilitation effects have largely employed multisensory contexts in which the stimuli were meaningfully related to the items targeting for remembering (e.g., pairing canonical sounds and images). Other studies have used unrelated stimuli as multisensory context. A third possible type of multisensory context is one that is environmentally related simply because the stimuli are often encountered together in the real world. We predicted that encountering such a multisensory context would also enhance memory through cross-modal associations, or representations relating to one's prior multisensory experience with that sort of stimuli in general. In two memory experiments, we used faces and voices of unfamiliar people as everyday stimuli individuals have substantial experience integrating the perceptual features of. We assigned participants to face- or voice-recognition groups and ensured that, during the study phase, half of the face or voice targets were encountered also with information in the other modality. Voices initially encoded along with faces were consistently remembered better, providing evidence that cross-modal associations could explain the observed multisensory facilitation.

多感官情境通常有助于感知和记忆。事实上,在多感官情境中对项目进行编码,甚至在严格的单感官测试中(即缺乏多感官情境时)也能提高记忆效果。之前持续发现这些多感官促进效应的研究大多采用了多感官情境,在这种情境中,刺激物与记忆的目标项目有意义上的关联(例如,将典型的声音和图像配对)。其他研究则使用无关刺激作为多感官情境。第三种可能的多感官情境是与环境相关的情境,原因很简单,因为这些刺激物在现实世界中经常一起出现。我们预测,遇到这种多感官情境时,也会通过跨模态联想或与之前对此类刺激的多感官体验相关的表征来增强记忆。在两项记忆实验中,我们使用了陌生人物的脸部和声音作为日常刺激物,这些刺激物对个人的感知特征具有丰富的整合经验。我们将参与者分配到人脸识别组或声音识别组,并确保在研究阶段,一半的人脸或声音目标也会遇到另一种模式的信息。最初与人脸一起编码的声音始终记得更牢,这证明跨模态关联可以解释所观察到的多感官促进作用。
{"title":"Cross-Modal Contributions to Episodic Memory for Voices.","authors":"Joshua R Tatz, Zehra F Peynircioğlu","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10116","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10116","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multisensory context often facilitates perception and memory. In fact, encoding items within a multisensory context can improve memory even on strictly unisensory tests (i.e., when the multisensory context is absent). Prior studies that have consistently found these multisensory facilitation effects have largely employed multisensory contexts in which the stimuli were meaningfully related to the items targeting for remembering (e.g., pairing canonical sounds and images). Other studies have used unrelated stimuli as multisensory context. A third possible type of multisensory context is one that is environmentally related simply because the stimuli are often encountered together in the real world. We predicted that encountering such a multisensory context would also enhance memory through cross-modal associations, or representations relating to one's prior multisensory experience with that sort of stimuli in general. In two memory experiments, we used faces and voices of unfamiliar people as everyday stimuli individuals have substantial experience integrating the perceptual features of. We assigned participants to face- or voice-recognition groups and ensured that, during the study phase, half of the face or voice targets were encountered also with information in the other modality. Voices initially encoded along with faces were consistently remembered better, providing evidence that cross-modal associations could explain the observed multisensory facilitation.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"47-74"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138808898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stationary Haptic Stimuli Do not Produce Ocular Accommodation in Most Individuals. 静止的触觉刺激在大多数个体中不会产生眼部调节。
IF 1.6 4区 心理学 Q3 BIOPHYSICS Pub Date : 2023-11-28 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-bja10115
Lawrence R Stark, Kim Shiraishi, Tyler Sommerfeld

This study aimed to determine the extent to which haptic stimuli can influence ocular accommodation, either alone or in combination with vision. Accommodation was measured objectively in 15 young adults as they read stationary targets containing Braille letters. These cards were presented at four distances in the range 20-50 cm. In the Touch condition, the participant read by touch with their dominant hand in a dark room. Afterward, they estimated card distance with their non-dominant hand. In the Vision condition, they read by sight binocularly without touch in a lighted room. In the Touch with Vision condition, they read by sight binocularly and with touch in a lighted room. Sensory modality had a significant overall effect on the slope of the accommodative stimulus-response function. The slope in the Touch condition was not significantly different from zero, even though depth perception from touch was accurate. Nevertheless, one atypical participant had a moderate accommodative slope in the Touch condition. The accommodative slope in the Touch condition was significantly poorer than in the Vision condition. The accommodative slopes in the Vision condition and Touch with Vision condition were not significantly different. For most individuals, haptic stimuli for stationary objects do not influence the accommodation response, alone or in combination with vision. These haptic stimuli provide accurate distance perception, thus questioning the general validity of Heath's model of proximal accommodation as driven by perceived distance. Instead, proximally induced accommodation relies on visual rather than touch stimuli.

本研究旨在确定触觉刺激对视觉调节的影响程度,无论是单独的还是与视觉结合的。在15名年轻人阅读包含盲文字母的固定目标时,客观地测量了他们的适应能力。这些卡片在20-50厘米的范围内以四种距离呈现。在触摸条件下,参与者在黑暗的房间里用惯用手触摸阅读。之后,他们用非惯用手估算出出牌的距离。在视觉条件下,他们在一个有灯光的房间里用双眼阅读,不需要触摸。在触觉与视觉的条件下,他们在一个有灯光的房间里用双眼和触觉阅读。感觉模态对调节刺激反应函数的斜率有显著的总体影响。尽管触觉深度感知是准确的,但触觉条件下的斜率与零没有显著差异。然而,一个非典型参与者在Touch条件下有中等调节斜率。触觉条件下的调节斜率明显低于视觉条件下的调节斜率。视觉条件和触觉条件下的调节斜率无显著差异。对于大多数人来说,对静止物体的触觉刺激,单独或与视觉结合,都不会影响调节反应。这些触觉刺激提供了准确的距离感知,从而质疑了Heath的近端调节模型由感知距离驱动的总体有效性。相反,近端诱导调节依赖于视觉刺激而不是触觉刺激。
{"title":"Stationary Haptic Stimuli Do not Produce Ocular Accommodation in Most Individuals.","authors":"Lawrence R Stark, Kim Shiraishi, Tyler Sommerfeld","doi":"10.1163/22134808-bja10115","DOIUrl":"10.1163/22134808-bja10115","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to determine the extent to which haptic stimuli can influence ocular accommodation, either alone or in combination with vision. Accommodation was measured objectively in 15 young adults as they read stationary targets containing Braille letters. These cards were presented at four distances in the range 20-50 cm. In the Touch condition, the participant read by touch with their dominant hand in a dark room. Afterward, they estimated card distance with their non-dominant hand. In the Vision condition, they read by sight binocularly without touch in a lighted room. In the Touch with Vision condition, they read by sight binocularly and with touch in a lighted room. Sensory modality had a significant overall effect on the slope of the accommodative stimulus-response function. The slope in the Touch condition was not significantly different from zero, even though depth perception from touch was accurate. Nevertheless, one atypical participant had a moderate accommodative slope in the Touch condition. The accommodative slope in the Touch condition was significantly poorer than in the Vision condition. The accommodative slopes in the Vision condition and Touch with Vision condition were not significantly different. For most individuals, haptic stimuli for stationary objects do not influence the accommodation response, alone or in combination with vision. These haptic stimuli provide accurate distance perception, thus questioning the general validity of Heath's model of proximal accommodation as driven by perceived distance. Instead, proximally induced accommodation relies on visual rather than touch stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":51298,"journal":{"name":"Multisensory Research","volume":" ","pages":"25-45"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2023-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138453050","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Multisensory Research
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1