首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Semiotics最新文献

英文 中文
Structural phenomenology: A reading of the early Husserl 结构现象学:早期胡塞尔的解读
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-10-12 DOI: 10.1515/cogsem-2018-2003
S. Aurora
Abstract The aim of this paper is to show that Edmund Husserl’s Logical Investigations should be regarded as one of the fundamental sources of structuralism. Husserl’s version of structuralism is, however, quite original in many respects. Indeed, what Husserl advocates in his works can be defined, following and expanding a line of research inaugurated by scholars like Elmar Holenstein and Giovanni Piana (see Holenstein, E. 1975. Roman Jakobsons phänomenologischer Strukturalismus. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp, Holenstein, E. 1976. Linguistik, Semiotik, Hermeneutik: Plädoyers für eine strukturale Phänomenologie. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp, Holenstein, E. In publication. Phenomenological Phenomenology of Language: Scattered Papers. Geneva: sdvig, Piana, G. 2013a. L'idea di uno strutturalismo fenomenologico. In G. Piana (ed.), Strutturalismo fenomenologico e psicologia della forma, 5–17. http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html (accessed 20 February 2018), Piana, G. 2013b. Momento figurale e qualità ghestaltica. In G. Piana (ed.), Strutturalismo fenomenologico e psicologia della forma, 101–119. http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html accessed 20 February 2018), in terms of phenomenological structuralism or of structural phenomenology.
摘要本文旨在说明胡塞尔的《逻辑研究》是结构主义理论的基本来源之一。然而,胡塞尔的结构主义版本在许多方面都是相当新颖的。的确,胡塞尔在他的著作中所倡导的东西是可以定义的,它遵循并扩展了Elmar Holenstein和Giovanni Piana等学者开创的研究路线(见Holenstein, E. 1975)。罗曼·雅各布森phänomenologischer结构主义。法兰克福:苏坎普,霍伦斯坦,E. 1976。语言学、符号学、解释学:Plädoyers f r eine struckturale Phänomenologie。法兰克福:Suhrkamp, Holenstein, E.出版中。语言现象学:零散的论文。日内瓦:sdvig, Piana, G. 2013a。结构主义现象学的概念。见G. Piana主编,《结构主义现象学与形式心理学》,第5-17页。http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html(2018年2月20日访问),Piana, G. 2013b。这是一种象征性的品质。见G. Piana主编,《结构主义现象学与形式心理学》,101-119页。http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html访问2018年2月20日),在现象学结构主义或结构现象学方面。
{"title":"Structural phenomenology: A reading of the early Husserl","authors":"S. Aurora","doi":"10.1515/cogsem-2018-2003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2018-2003","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The aim of this paper is to show that Edmund Husserl’s Logical Investigations should be regarded as one of the fundamental sources of structuralism. Husserl’s version of structuralism is, however, quite original in many respects. Indeed, what Husserl advocates in his works can be defined, following and expanding a line of research inaugurated by scholars like Elmar Holenstein and Giovanni Piana (see Holenstein, E. 1975. Roman Jakobsons phänomenologischer Strukturalismus. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp, Holenstein, E. 1976. Linguistik, Semiotik, Hermeneutik: Plädoyers für eine strukturale Phänomenologie. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp, Holenstein, E. In publication. Phenomenological Phenomenology of Language: Scattered Papers. Geneva: sdvig, Piana, G. 2013a. L'idea di uno strutturalismo fenomenologico. In G. Piana (ed.), Strutturalismo fenomenologico e psicologia della forma, 5–17. http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html (accessed 20 February 2018), Piana, G. 2013b. Momento figurale e qualità ghestaltica. In G. Piana (ed.), Strutturalismo fenomenologico e psicologia della forma, 101–119. http://www.lulu.com/shop/giovanni-piana/strutturalismo-fenomenologico-e-psicologia-della-forma/paperback/product-21332317.html accessed 20 February 2018), in terms of phenomenological structuralism or of structural phenomenology.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"12 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89942004","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Contextualising screen violence: An integrative approach toward explaining of the functions of violent narrative events in audiovisual media 情境化屏幕暴力:一种解释视听媒体中暴力叙事事件功能的综合方法
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-10-02 DOI: 10.1515/cogsem-2018-2001
Chiao-I Tseng
Abstract This article aims to examine the narrative impacts and social influences of screen violence in audiovisual media. It suggests an integrative approach to synthesising the recent research findings in different disciplines such as cognitive science, media studies, neuroscience, and social semiotic theories. Based on the theoretical synthesis of narrative effects and persuasive functions, this paper establishes a method for analysing the contextualisation of violent events. In particular, the analytical method focuses on the two main narrative mechanisms for contextualising violent events, justifications of characters’ motivations for using violence and depictions of consequences. This article will apply the method to elucidate how different kinds of contextualisation yield different types of narrative impacts, persuasive potentials, and the ways in which social, political, and ideological issues can be learnt. Furthermore, a typology of characters’ motivations is also provided, which are often used for justifying the characters’ violent actions in audiovisual narratives. This paper also unravels how genre expectations are closely related to narrative functions of screen violence, particularly how genre shapes the viewers’ prediction and interpretation of violent events. Finally, the methods for motivation analysis of violent narrative events are extended to examine a particular genre of interactive audiovisual texts — empathy games.
摘要本文旨在探讨视听媒体中屏幕暴力的叙事影响和社会影响。它提出了一种综合不同学科的最新研究成果的综合方法,如认知科学、媒体研究、神经科学和社会符号学理论。基于叙事效果和说服功能的理论综合,本文建立了一种分析暴力事件语境化的方法。特别是,分析方法侧重于将暴力事件置于情境中的两种主要叙事机制,即角色使用暴力动机的正当性以及对后果的描述。本文将运用该方法来阐明不同类型的语境化如何产生不同类型的叙事影响、说服潜力,以及社会、政治和意识形态问题的学习方式。此外,还提供了角色动机的类型,这通常用于证明视听叙事中角色的暴力行为是合理的。本文还揭示了类型预期如何与屏幕暴力的叙事功能密切相关,特别是类型如何塑造观众对暴力事件的预测和解释。最后,本文将暴力叙事事件的动机分析方法扩展到一种特殊类型的互动式视听文本——移情游戏。
{"title":"Contextualising screen violence: An integrative approach toward explaining of the functions of violent narrative events in audiovisual media","authors":"Chiao-I Tseng","doi":"10.1515/cogsem-2018-2001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2018-2001","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This article aims to examine the narrative impacts and social influences of screen violence in audiovisual media. It suggests an integrative approach to synthesising the recent research findings in different disciplines such as cognitive science, media studies, neuroscience, and social semiotic theories. Based on the theoretical synthesis of narrative effects and persuasive functions, this paper establishes a method for analysing the contextualisation of violent events. In particular, the analytical method focuses on the two main narrative mechanisms for contextualising violent events, justifications of characters’ motivations for using violence and depictions of consequences. This article will apply the method to elucidate how different kinds of contextualisation yield different types of narrative impacts, persuasive potentials, and the ways in which social, political, and ideological issues can be learnt. Furthermore, a typology of characters’ motivations is also provided, which are often used for justifying the characters’ violent actions in audiovisual narratives. This paper also unravels how genre expectations are closely related to narrative functions of screen violence, particularly how genre shapes the viewers’ prediction and interpretation of violent events. Finally, the methods for motivation analysis of violent narrative events are extended to examine a particular genre of interactive audiovisual texts — empathy games.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"74 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86297438","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Upright posture and the meaning of meronymy: A synthesis of metaphoric and analytic accounts 直立姿势与异名的意义:隐喻与分析的综合
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0003
Jamin Pelkey
Abstract Cross-linguistic strategies for mapping lexical and spatial relations from body partonym systems to external object meronymies (as in English ‘table leg,’ ‘mountain face’) have attracted substantial research and debate over the past three decades. Due to the systematic mappings, lexical productivity, and geometric complexities of body-based meronymies found in many Mesoamerican languages, the region has become focal for these discussions, prominently including contrastive accounts of the phenomenon in Zapotec and Tzeltal, leading researchers to question whether such systems should be explained as global metaphorical mappings from bodily source to target holonym or as vector mappings of shape and axis generated “algorithmically.” I propose a synthesis of these accounts in this paper by drawing on the species-specific cognitive affordances of human upright posture grounded in the reorganization of the anatomical planes, with a special emphasis on antisymmetrical relations that emerge between arm-leg and face-groin antinomies cross-culturally. Whereas Levinson argues that the internal geometry of objects “stripped of their bodily associations” (1994: 821) is sufficient to account for Tzeltal meronymy, making metaphorical explanations entirely unnecessary, I propose a more powerful, elegant explanation of Tzeltal meronymic mapping that affirms both the geometric-analytic and the global-metaphorical nature of Tzeltal meaning construal. I do this by demonstrating that the “algorithm” in question arises from the phenomenology of movement and correlative body memories — an experiential ground that generates a culturally selected pair of inverse contrastive paradigm sets with marked and unmarked membership emerging antithetically relative to the transverse anatomical plane. These relations are then selected diagrammatically for the classification of object orientations according to systematic geometric iconicities. Results not only serve to clarify the case in question but also point to the relatively untapped potential that upright posture holds for theorizing the emergence of human cognition, highlighting in the process the nature, origins and theoretical validity of markedness and double scope conceptual integration.
在过去的三十年里,将身体同音系统的词汇和空间关系映射到外部物体同音系统(如英语中的“table leg”、“mountain face”)的跨语言策略引起了大量的研究和争论。由于在许多中美洲语言中发现的系统映射、词汇生产力和基于身体的同义词的几何复杂性,该地区已成为这些讨论的焦点,其中包括萨波特克语和泽塔尔语中对这种现象的对比描述,这导致研究人员质疑,这种系统是否应该被解释为从身体来源到目标全义词的全球隐喻映射,还是作为“算法”生成的形状和轴的矢量映射。我在本文中提出了一种综合这些说法的方法,利用基于解剖平面重组的人类直立姿势的物种特异性认知能力,特别强调手臂-腿和面部-腹股沟二律背反之间跨文化出现的不对称关系。Levinson认为,物体的内部几何“剥离了它们的身体联系”(1994:821)足以解释Tzeltal的双名化,使隐喻解释完全没有必要,我提出了一个更有力、更优雅的Tzeltal双名化映射的解释,它肯定了Tzeltal意义解释的几何解析和全局隐喻性质。为此,我论证了问题中的“算法”产生于运动现象学和相关的身体记忆——这是一个经验基础,它产生了文化选择的一对反向对比范式集,其中有标记和无标记的成员相对于横向解剖平面相反地出现。然后,根据系统的几何象似性,用图表选择这些关系用于对象方向的分类。研究结果不仅有助于澄清所讨论的问题,而且还指出直立姿势在人类认知出现的理论化方面具有相对未开发的潜力,在这一过程中突出了标记性和双范围概念整合的本质、起源和理论有效性。
{"title":"Upright posture and the meaning of meronymy: A synthesis of metaphoric and analytic accounts","authors":"Jamin Pelkey","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0003","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Cross-linguistic strategies for mapping lexical and spatial relations from body partonym systems to external object meronymies (as in English ‘table leg,’ ‘mountain face’) have attracted substantial research and debate over the past three decades. Due to the systematic mappings, lexical productivity, and geometric complexities of body-based meronymies found in many Mesoamerican languages, the region has become focal for these discussions, prominently including contrastive accounts of the phenomenon in Zapotec and Tzeltal, leading researchers to question whether such systems should be explained as global metaphorical mappings from bodily source to target holonym or as vector mappings of shape and axis generated “algorithmically.” I propose a synthesis of these accounts in this paper by drawing on the species-specific cognitive affordances of human upright posture grounded in the reorganization of the anatomical planes, with a special emphasis on antisymmetrical relations that emerge between arm-leg and face-groin antinomies cross-culturally. Whereas Levinson argues that the internal geometry of objects “stripped of their bodily associations” (1994: 821) is sufficient to account for Tzeltal meronymy, making metaphorical explanations entirely unnecessary, I propose a more powerful, elegant explanation of Tzeltal meronymic mapping that affirms both the geometric-analytic and the global-metaphorical nature of Tzeltal meaning construal. I do this by demonstrating that the “algorithm” in question arises from the phenomenology of movement and correlative body memories — an experiential ground that generates a culturally selected pair of inverse contrastive paradigm sets with marked and unmarked membership emerging antithetically relative to the transverse anatomical plane. These relations are then selected diagrammatically for the classification of object orientations according to systematic geometric iconicities. Results not only serve to clarify the case in question but also point to the relatively untapped potential that upright posture holds for theorizing the emergence of human cognition, highlighting in the process the nature, origins and theoretical validity of markedness and double scope conceptual integration.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"99 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84103811","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Meaning making from life to language:The Semiotic Hierarchy and phenomenology 从生活到语言的意义建构:符号学层次与现象学
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0001
J. Zlatev
Abstract The paper rethinks a proposal for a unified cognitive semiotic framework, The Semiotic Hierarchy, in explicitly phenomenological terms, following above all the work of Merleau-Ponty. The main changes to the earlier formulation of the theory are the following. First, the claim that a general concept of meaning can be understood as the value-based relationship between the subject and the world is shown to correspond to the most fundamental concept of phenomenology: intentionality, understood as “openness to the world.” Second, the rather strict nature of the original hierarchy of meaning levels made the model rather static and one-directional, thus resembling an old-fashioned scala naturae. Reformulating the relationship between the levels in terms of the dynamical notion of Fundierung avoids this pitfall. Third, the phenomenological analysis allows, somewhat paradoxically, both a greater number of levels (life, subjectivity, intersubjectivity, sign function, language) and less discrete borders between these. Fourth, there is an intimate relation between (levels and kinds of) intentionality and normativity, making the normativity of language a special case. Fifth, to each level of meaning corresponds a dialectics of spontaneity and sedimentation, with corresponding normative structures (e.g., habits, emotions, conventions, signs and grammar) both emerging from and constraining, but not determining, subject-world interactions. Sixth and finally, the analysis follows the basic phenomenological principle to examine the phenomena without theoretical preconceptions, and without premature explanations. This implies a focus on human experience, even when dealing with the “biological” level of meaning, with the possibility of extending the analysis to non-human subjects through empathy. The intention is that this phenomenologically interpreted version of the Semiotic Hierarchy may serve as a useful tool against any kind of meaning reductionism, whether biological, mental, social or linguistic.
摘要本文在梅洛-庞蒂的基础上,从现象学的角度重新思考了统一的认知符号学框架——符号学层次。该理论的早期表述的主要变化如下。首先,一般意义概念可以被理解为主体与世界之间基于价值的关系,这一主张与现象学最基本的概念相对应:意向性,被理解为“向世界开放”。其次,原始的意义层次结构相当严格的性质使得模型相当静态和单向,因此类似于老式的自然scala。根据动态的基础概念来重新表述关卡之间的关系可以避免这个陷阱。第三,现象学分析允许更多的层次(生命、主体性、主体间性、符号功能、语言)和它们之间更少的离散边界,这有点矛盾。第四,意向性与规范性(层次和种类)之间有着密切的关系,这使得语言的规范性成为一个特例。第五,每一层次的意义都对应着自发性和沉淀性的辩证法,以及相应的规范结构(如习惯、情感、习俗、符号和语法),它们都是从主体世界的相互作用中产生、制约而不是决定的。第六,也是最后一点,分析遵循基本的现象学原则,在没有理论先入为主的情况下检查现象,也没有过早的解释。这意味着对人类经验的关注,即使在处理意义的“生物学”层面时,也有可能通过移情将分析扩展到非人类主体。其目的是,符号学层次的现象学解释版本可以作为一个有用的工具,反对任何形式的意义还原论,无论是生物的,心理的,社会的还是语言的。
{"title":"Meaning making from life to language:The Semiotic Hierarchy and phenomenology","authors":"J. Zlatev","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0001","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The paper rethinks a proposal for a unified cognitive semiotic framework, The Semiotic Hierarchy, in explicitly phenomenological terms, following above all the work of Merleau-Ponty. The main changes to the earlier formulation of the theory are the following. First, the claim that a general concept of meaning can be understood as the value-based relationship between the subject and the world is shown to correspond to the most fundamental concept of phenomenology: intentionality, understood as “openness to the world.” Second, the rather strict nature of the original hierarchy of meaning levels made the model rather static and one-directional, thus resembling an old-fashioned scala naturae. Reformulating the relationship between the levels in terms of the dynamical notion of Fundierung avoids this pitfall. Third, the phenomenological analysis allows, somewhat paradoxically, both a greater number of levels (life, subjectivity, intersubjectivity, sign function, language) and less discrete borders between these. Fourth, there is an intimate relation between (levels and kinds of) intentionality and normativity, making the normativity of language a special case. Fifth, to each level of meaning corresponds a dialectics of spontaneity and sedimentation, with corresponding normative structures (e.g., habits, emotions, conventions, signs and grammar) both emerging from and constraining, but not determining, subject-world interactions. Sixth and finally, the analysis follows the basic phenomenological principle to examine the phenomena without theoretical preconceptions, and without premature explanations. This implies a focus on human experience, even when dealing with the “biological” level of meaning, with the possibility of extending the analysis to non-human subjects through empathy. The intention is that this phenomenologically interpreted version of the Semiotic Hierarchy may serve as a useful tool against any kind of meaning reductionism, whether biological, mental, social or linguistic.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"114 2 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91022829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 32
Gestures as image schemas and force gestalts: A dynamic systems approach augmented with motion-capture data analyses 手势作为图像图式和力格式塔:动态系统方法与动作捕捉数据分析增强
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0002
I. Mittelberg
Abstract Embodied image schemas are central to experientialist accounts of meaning-making. Research from several disciplines has evidenced their pervasiveness in motivating form and meaning in both literal and figurative expressions across diverse semiotic systems and art forms (e.g., Gibbs and Colston; Hampe; Johnson; Lakoff; and Mandler). This paper aims to highlight structural similarities between, on the one hand, dynamic image schemas and force schemas and, on the other, hand shapes and gestural movements. Such flexible correspondences between conceptual and gestural schematicity are assumed to partly stem from experiential bases shared by incrementally internalized conceptual structures and the repeated gestural (re-) enacting of bodily actions as well as more abstract semantic primitives (Lakoff). Gestures typically consist of evanescent, metonymically reduced hand configurations, motion onsets, or movement traces that minimally suggest, for instance, a PATH, the idea of CONTAINMENT, an IN-OUT spatial relation, or the momentary loss of emotional BALANCE. So, while physical in nature, gestures often emerge as rather schematic gestalts that, as such, have the capacity to vividly convey essential semantic and pragmatic aspects of high relevance to the speaker. It is further argued that gesturally instantiated image schemas and force dynamics are inherently meaningful structures that typically underlie more complex semantic and pragmatic processes involving, for instance, metonymy, metaphor, and frames. First, I discuss previous work on how image schemas, force gestalts, and mimetic schemas may underpin hand gestures and body postures. Drawing on Gibbs’ dynamic systems account of image schemas, I then introduce an array of tendencies in gestural image schema enactments: body-inherent/self-oriented (body as image-schematic structure; forces acting upon the body); environment-oriented (material culture including spatial structures), and interlocutor-oriented (intersubjective understanding). Adopting a dynamic systems perspective (e.g.,Thompson and Varela) thus puts the focus on how image schemas and force gestalts that operate in gesture may function as cognitive-semiotic organizing principles that underpin a) the physical and cognitive self-regulation of speakers; b) how they interact with the (virtual) environment while talking; and c) intersubjective instances of resonance and understanding between interlocutors or between an artwork and its beholder. Examples of these patterns are enriched by video and motion-capture data, showing how numeric kinetic data allow one to measure the temporal and spatial dimensions of gestural articulations and to visualize movement traces.
抽象具身意象图式是经验主义意义形成理论的核心。来自多个学科的研究证明了它们在各种符号系统和艺术形式的字面和比喻表达中的激励形式和意义的普遍存在(例如,Gibbs和Colston;Hampe;约翰逊;Lakoff;和Mandler)。本文旨在强调动态形象图式和力量图式之间的结构相似性,以及手部形状和手势运动之间的结构相似性。概念图式和手势图式之间的这种灵活对应被认为部分源于逐渐内化的概念结构和重复的身体动作的手势(再)制定以及更抽象的语义原语所共享的经验基础(Lakoff)。手势通常由转瞬即逝的、转喻式地减少的手部配置、动作开始或运动痕迹组成,这些痕迹最低限度地表明,例如,PATH、包容的想法、IN-OUT空间关系或瞬间失去情感平衡。因此,虽然手势本质上是身体的,但它往往是一种示意图式的完形,因此,它有能力生动地传达与说话者高度相关的基本语义和语用方面。进一步认为,手势实例化的图像图式和力动力学本质上是有意义的结构,通常是更复杂的语义和语用过程的基础,例如转喻、隐喻和框架。首先,我讨论了先前关于图像图式、力格式塔和模仿图式如何支撑手势和身体姿势的工作。根据吉布斯对意象图式的动态系统解释,我随后介绍了手势意象图式制定中的一系列倾向:身体固有/自我导向(身体作为意象图式结构;作用于物体的力);环境导向(包括空间结构在内的物质文化)和对话者导向(主体间理解)。因此,采用动态系统视角(如Thompson和Varela)将重点放在手势中操作的图像图式和力格式塔如何作为认知符号学组织原则发挥作用,这些原则支持a)说话者的身体和认知自我调节;B)他们在交谈时如何与(虚拟)环境互动;c)对话者之间或艺术品与观者之间的共鸣和理解的主体间实例。视频和动作捕捉数据丰富了这些模式的示例,显示了数值动力学数据如何允许人们测量手势关节的时间和空间维度并可视化运动轨迹。
{"title":"Gestures as image schemas and force gestalts: A dynamic systems approach augmented with motion-capture data analyses","authors":"I. Mittelberg","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0002","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Embodied image schemas are central to experientialist accounts of meaning-making. Research from several disciplines has evidenced their pervasiveness in motivating form and meaning in both literal and figurative expressions across diverse semiotic systems and art forms (e.g., Gibbs and Colston; Hampe; Johnson; Lakoff; and Mandler). This paper aims to highlight structural similarities between, on the one hand, dynamic image schemas and force schemas and, on the other, hand shapes and gestural movements. Such flexible correspondences between conceptual and gestural schematicity are assumed to partly stem from experiential bases shared by incrementally internalized conceptual structures and the repeated gestural (re-) enacting of bodily actions as well as more abstract semantic primitives (Lakoff). Gestures typically consist of evanescent, metonymically reduced hand configurations, motion onsets, or movement traces that minimally suggest, for instance, a PATH, the idea of CONTAINMENT, an IN-OUT spatial relation, or the momentary loss of emotional BALANCE. So, while physical in nature, gestures often emerge as rather schematic gestalts that, as such, have the capacity to vividly convey essential semantic and pragmatic aspects of high relevance to the speaker. It is further argued that gesturally instantiated image schemas and force dynamics are inherently meaningful structures that typically underlie more complex semantic and pragmatic processes involving, for instance, metonymy, metaphor, and frames. First, I discuss previous work on how image schemas, force gestalts, and mimetic schemas may underpin hand gestures and body postures. Drawing on Gibbs’ dynamic systems account of image schemas, I then introduce an array of tendencies in gestural image schema enactments: body-inherent/self-oriented (body as image-schematic structure; forces acting upon the body); environment-oriented (material culture including spatial structures), and interlocutor-oriented (intersubjective understanding). Adopting a dynamic systems perspective (e.g.,Thompson and Varela) thus puts the focus on how image schemas and force gestalts that operate in gesture may function as cognitive-semiotic organizing principles that underpin a) the physical and cognitive self-regulation of speakers; b) how they interact with the (virtual) environment while talking; and c) intersubjective instances of resonance and understanding between interlocutors or between an artwork and its beholder. Examples of these patterns are enriched by video and motion-capture data, showing how numeric kinetic data allow one to measure the temporal and spatial dimensions of gestural articulations and to visualize movement traces.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"47 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90638366","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 33
Temporality of sense-making in narrative interactions 叙事互动中意义建构的时间性
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0007
Yanna Popova, E. Cuffari
Abstract Human experience is inherently temporal and involves placing events, symbols, and actions in a temporal scheme. This article deals with a specific aspect of temporal experience as it relates to reading and experiencing narrative fiction. Within an enactive understanding of human cognition, we propose a view of literary reading as a process of participatory sense-making between a reader and a storyteller. The enactive theory of participatory sense-making maintains that agents, by enacting their own sense-making, directly and partially constitute the sense-making of other agents. Sense-makers in interaction navigate two orders of normativity: their own and that of the interaction itself. Linguistic sense-making (languaging) opens up further possibilities for understanding complex spatially and temporally distributed forms of social interactions such as narrative interactions. Reading a narrative is one such example of mutually constituted navigation between an interaction dynamic and interactors’ sense-making. The reader completes and co-authors emergent textual meaning and a textually emerging storyteller guides and anticipates the multiple temporal displacements, realized linguistically, that a reader has to experience in the process of reading. We explore the participatory structure of a narrative through its temporal unfolding and the specific, non-linear nature of the temporal dynamics of interacting with a storytelling agency. In particular, narrative interactions are seen as modulations in the pacing of a given narrative’s unfolding. It is suggested that the reader’s enactment of such temporally realized pacings constitutes a better description of narrative immersion than its traditional understanding as a simulation of spatial situatedness.
人类的经验本质上是暂时的,包括将事件、符号和行动放置在一个暂时的方案中。这篇文章涉及时间体验的一个具体方面,因为它涉及到阅读和体验叙事小说。在对人类认知的主动理解中,我们提出了一种文学阅读的观点,即读者和讲故事者之间的参与性意义形成过程。参与性意义建构的行动理论认为,通过实施自己的意义建构,行动者直接和部分地构成了其他行动者的意义建构。交互中的意义制造者在两个规范性秩序中导航:他们自己的秩序和交互本身的秩序。语言意义构建(语言)为理解复杂的空间和时间分布的社会互动形式(如叙事互动)提供了进一步的可能性。阅读叙述就是在互动动态和互动者的意义创造之间相互构成导航的一个例子。读者完成并共同创作突现的文本意义,而文本突现的说书人引导并预期读者在阅读过程中必须经历的多重时间位移,并在语言上实现。我们通过叙事的时间展开和与讲故事机构互动的时间动态的具体、非线性性质来探索叙事的参与性结构。特别是,叙述互动被看作是给定叙述展开节奏的调整。作者认为,读者对这种暂时实现的节奏的设定比传统的对空间情境性的模拟更好地描述了叙事沉浸。
{"title":"Temporality of sense-making in narrative interactions","authors":"Yanna Popova, E. Cuffari","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0007","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Human experience is inherently temporal and involves placing events, symbols, and actions in a temporal scheme. This article deals with a specific aspect of temporal experience as it relates to reading and experiencing narrative fiction. Within an enactive understanding of human cognition, we propose a view of literary reading as a process of participatory sense-making between a reader and a storyteller. The enactive theory of participatory sense-making maintains that agents, by enacting their own sense-making, directly and partially constitute the sense-making of other agents. Sense-makers in interaction navigate two orders of normativity: their own and that of the interaction itself. Linguistic sense-making (languaging) opens up further possibilities for understanding complex spatially and temporally distributed forms of social interactions such as narrative interactions. Reading a narrative is one such example of mutually constituted navigation between an interaction dynamic and interactors’ sense-making. The reader completes and co-authors emergent textual meaning and a textually emerging storyteller guides and anticipates the multiple temporal displacements, realized linguistically, that a reader has to experience in the process of reading. We explore the participatory structure of a narrative through its temporal unfolding and the specific, non-linear nature of the temporal dynamics of interacting with a storytelling agency. In particular, narrative interactions are seen as modulations in the pacing of a given narrative’s unfolding. It is suggested that the reader’s enactment of such temporally realized pacings constitutes a better description of narrative immersion than its traditional understanding as a simulation of spatial situatedness.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87302221","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Retraction of: The rhetorics of fictiveinteraction in advertising: The case for imagined direct speech in argumentation 广告中虚构互动的修辞学:论证中想象的直接引语的案例
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0008
Line Brandt
Retraction of: Brandt, Line. November 2016. The rhetorics of fictive interaction in advertising: The case for imagined direct speech in argumentation. Volume 9(2), pages 149–182. (DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-20160006). This article has been withdrawn due to copyright infringement of the article Brandt, Line & Esther Pascual. 2016. “Say hello to this ad” – The persuasive rhetoric of fictive interaction in marketing”. In Esther Pascual and Sergeiy Sandler (eds.), The Conversation Frame. Forms and functions of fictive interaction, 303–322. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
收回:勃兰特,莱恩。2016年11月。广告中有效互动的修辞学:论证中想象直接引语的案例。第9卷(2),149-182页。(DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem - 20160006)。由于侵犯了Brandt, Line & Esther Pascual. 2016这篇文章的版权,本文已被撤回。“向这个广告问好”——营销中有效互动的有说服力的修辞。见埃斯特·帕斯卡尔和谢尔盖·桑德勒主编的《对话框架》。有效互动的形式和功能,303-322。阿姆斯特丹:约翰·本杰明。
{"title":"Retraction of: The rhetorics of fictiveinteraction in advertising: The case for imagined direct speech in argumentation","authors":"Line Brandt","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0008","url":null,"abstract":"Retraction of: Brandt, Line. November 2016. The rhetorics of fictive interaction in advertising: The case for imagined direct speech in argumentation. Volume 9(2), pages 149–182. (DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-20160006). This article has been withdrawn due to copyright infringement of the article Brandt, Line & Esther Pascual. 2016. “Say hello to this ad” – The persuasive rhetoric of fictive interaction in marketing”. In Esther Pascual and Sergeiy Sandler (eds.), The Conversation Frame. Forms and functions of fictive interaction, 303–322. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"74 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74161823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contrasting attention to mutual knowledge in English and Mopan Mayan conversation: Schooling, orality, and cultural cosmology 英语和磨盘玛雅语对话中对相互知识的对比关注:学校教育、口语和文化宇宙观
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-05-01 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0004
E. Danziger
Abstract Classroom training for attention to utterance recipients’ likely states of knowledge is useful in order to compensate for the situations of reduced co-presence that characterize literate communication at a distance. But many aspects of the Mopan (Mayan) philosophy of language resonate instead with non-schooled practices of Mopan socialization that support oral and not literate transmission of knowledge. In Mopan, everyday speech and action are evaluated with reference to a more-than-human moral order in which what counts is fidelity to ancestral prescriptions rather than to one’s own or others’ momentary mental states. Such cultural differences in beliefs about the (non-)importance of mental states are known to enter into institutionalized moralities such as those governing legal decisions or religious obligations. At the same time, many unconscious and embodied aspects of meaning-making in interaction are clearly conducted without apparent input from differing cultural beliefs. The present study shows how cultural attitudes about meaning-making play out at a level intermediate between these two apparently contradictory extremes. Mopan farmers and US English university students engaged with an interactional matching task in which visual common ground is occluded and speakers must describe a photograph in such a way that the listener succeeds in picking out that very photograph from among a set of similar ones. Schooled US English participants rarely describe any attributes of the photos other than their minimum distinguishing features, and they almost always mention those features. In contrast, Mopan participants often construe the interactional task as one that requires accurate and complete description of single items one at a time, rather than requiring identification of key attributes that will uniquely identify the target referent in its current context to a particular listener. Significant differences in strategic approaches to real-time construction of conversational reference is thus shown to correspond to contrasting cultural belief systems about the making of meaning, themselves related to literate versus oral modes of knowledge transmission.
课堂训练对话语接受者可能的知识状态的关注是有用的,以弥补远距离文学交流中共同存在减少的情况。但是,磨盘(玛雅)语言哲学的许多方面与磨盘社会化的非学校实践产生了共鸣,这些实践支持口头而不是文字的知识传播。在《磨盘》中,每天的言语和行为都是根据一种超越人类的道德秩序来评估的,在这种道德秩序中,重要的是对祖先处方的忠诚,而不是对自己或他人一时的精神状态的忠诚。这种关于精神状态(非)重要性的信仰的文化差异被认为会进入制度化的道德,例如那些管理法律决定或宗教义务的道德。与此同时,在互动中,许多无意识的和具体化的意义创造方面都是在没有不同文化信仰的明显输入的情况下清晰地进行的。目前的研究表明,关于意义形成的文化态度是如何在这两个明显矛盾的极端之间发挥作用的。磨盘农民和美国英语大学的学生参与了一项互动匹配任务,在这项任务中,视觉上的共同点被屏蔽,说话者必须以一种方式描述一张照片,使听者能够从一组相似的照片中成功地挑选出这张照片。受过美国英语教育的参与者除了最基本的特征外,很少描述照片的其他属性,而且他们几乎总是提到这些特征。相反,Mopan参与者通常将交互任务解释为需要一次对单个项目进行准确和完整的描述,而不是需要识别关键属性,这些属性将在当前上下文中唯一地标识特定侦听器的目标引用。因此,实时构建会话参考的策略方法的显著差异表明,它们对应于关于意义形成的不同文化信仰体系,它们本身与知识传播的读写模式和口头模式有关。
{"title":"Contrasting attention to mutual knowledge in English and Mopan Mayan conversation: Schooling, orality, and cultural cosmology","authors":"E. Danziger","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0004","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Classroom training for attention to utterance recipients’ likely states of knowledge is useful in order to compensate for the situations of reduced co-presence that characterize literate communication at a distance. But many aspects of the Mopan (Mayan) philosophy of language resonate instead with non-schooled practices of Mopan socialization that support oral and not literate transmission of knowledge. In Mopan, everyday speech and action are evaluated with reference to a more-than-human moral order in which what counts is fidelity to ancestral prescriptions rather than to one’s own or others’ momentary mental states. Such cultural differences in beliefs about the (non-)importance of mental states are known to enter into institutionalized moralities such as those governing legal decisions or religious obligations. At the same time, many unconscious and embodied aspects of meaning-making in interaction are clearly conducted without apparent input from differing cultural beliefs. The present study shows how cultural attitudes about meaning-making play out at a level intermediate between these two apparently contradictory extremes. Mopan farmers and US English university students engaged with an interactional matching task in which visual common ground is occluded and speakers must describe a photograph in such a way that the listener succeeds in picking out that very photograph from among a set of similar ones. Schooled US English participants rarely describe any attributes of the photos other than their minimum distinguishing features, and they almost always mention those features. In contrast, Mopan participants often construe the interactional task as one that requires accurate and complete description of single items one at a time, rather than requiring identification of key attributes that will uniquely identify the target referent in its current context to a particular listener. Significant differences in strategic approaches to real-time construction of conversational reference is thus shown to correspond to contrasting cultural belief systems about the making of meaning, themselves related to literate versus oral modes of knowledge transmission.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84236881","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ecological meaning, linguistic meaning, and interactivity 生态意义、语言意义和互动性
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-04-21 DOI: 10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0005
S. Steffensen, M. Harvey
Abstract Human language is extraordinarily meaningful. Well-spoken or well-written passages can evoke our deepest emotions and elicit all manner of conscious and subconscious reactions. This is usually taken to be an insurmountable explanatory challenge for ecological approaches to cognitive science, the primary tools of which concern coordination dynamics in organism-environment systems. Recent work (Pattee, H. H. & J. Rączaszek-Leonardi 2012. Laws, Language, and Life. Dordrecht: Springer) has made headway in describing the meaningfulness of linguistic units — the kind of meaning that we perceive as mediated by specific symbols — within an ecological framework, by building an account based on Howard Pattee’s conceptualization of symbols as physical, replicable, historically-selected constraints on the dynamics of self-organizing systems (Pattee, H. H. 1969. How does a molecule become a message?. Developmental Biology 3(supplemental). 1016; Pattee, H. H. 1972. Laws and constraints, symbols and languages. In C. H. Waddington (ed.), Towards a Theoretical Biology, 248–258. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press). In order to propose an “interactivity-based” approach to linguistic meaning, this paper takes the following steps: first, it rejects the view of linguistic meaning as fully independent from organism-environment interactions, as exemplified by formal approaches in philosophical semantics. Second, it presents a cutting-edge example of an ecological approach to symbols, namely Joanna Rączaszek-Leonardi’s (Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2009. Symbols as constraints: The structuring role of dynamics and self-organization in natural language. Pragmatics and Cognition 17(3). 653–676. DOI:10.1075/pandc.17.3.09ras; Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2016. How does a word become a message? An illustration on a developmental time-scale. New Ideas in Psychology 42, Supplement C: 46–55. DOI:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.08.001) version of Pattee’s symbols-as-constraints model. Third, it reviews and critiques a recent attempt (Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., I. Nomikou, K. J. Rohlfing & T. W. Deacon. 2018. Language development from an ecological perspective: Ecologically valid ways to abstract symbols. Ecological Psychology 30(1). 39–73) to integrate the symbols-as-constraints model with Terrence Deacon, T. W. 1997. The Symbolic Species. New York: W. W. Norton and Company; Deacon, T. W. 2011. The symbol concept. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, 393–405. Oxford: Oxford University Press) semiotic view of symbols, arguing that the properties ascribed to linguistic symbols, both by Deacon and very widely throughout the cognitive sciences, are not properties of individual instances of linguistic action. Rather, they belong to a particular mode of description that draws generalizations across the phenomenological experience of many language users. Finally, it lays out the core components of a novel “interactivity-based” approach to linguistic
人类的语言是非常有意义的。说得好或写得好的文章可以唤起我们最深的情感,并引起各种有意识和潜意识的反应。这通常被认为是认知科学的生态方法无法克服的解释挑战,认知科学的主要工具是关注生物-环境系统中的协调动力学。近期作品(Pattee, h.h. & J. Rączaszek-Leonardi 2012。法律、语言和生活。Dordrecht: Springer)在描述语言单位的意义方面取得了进展——我们认为这种意义是由特定的符号调解的——在生态框架内,通过建立一个基于霍华德·帕蒂(Howard Pattee)的符号概念的解释,作为自组织系统动态的物理的、可复制的、历史选择的约束(帕蒂,h.h. 1969)。一个分子是如何变成信息的?发育生物学3(补编)。1016;帕蒂,h.h. 1972。法律和约束,符号和语言。在c.h.沃丁顿(编),走向理论生物学,248-258。爱丁堡:爱丁堡大学出版社)。为了提出一种“基于互动性”的语言意义研究方法,本文采取了以下步骤:首先,它拒绝了语言意义完全独立于有机体-环境相互作用的观点,例如哲学语义学中的形式方法。其次,它提出了一个前沿的生态方法符号的例子,即乔安娜Rączaszek-Leonardi (Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2009)。符号作为约束:动态和自组织在自然语言中的结构作用。语用学与认知17(3)。653 - 676。DOI: 10.1075 / pandc.17.3.09ras;Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2016。一个词是如何变成信息的?一个关于发展时间尺度的插图。心理学新思想[j],增刊C: 46-55。DOI:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.08.001) Pattee的符号即约束模型的版本。第三,它回顾和批评了最近的尝试(Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., I. Nomikou, K. J. Rohlfing & T. W. Deacon. 2018)。生态视角下的语言发展:抽象符号的生态有效方式。生态心理学30(1)。(39-73)与Terrence Deacon, t.w. 1997整合符号作为约束模型。象征物种。纽约:w.w.诺顿出版社;迪肯,t.w. 2011。符号概念。在M.塔勒曼和k.r.吉布森(编),语言进化的牛津手册,393-405。(牛津:牛津大学出版社)符号的符号学观点,认为Deacon和整个认知科学赋予语言符号的属性,并不是语言行为的个别实例的属性。相反,它们属于一种特殊的描述模式,这种模式可以概括许多语言使用者的现象学经验。最后,它列出了一种新颖的“基于交互性”的语言意义研究方法的核心组成部分。根据这种观点,人类参与了持续的、超灵活的娱乐和训练,对声音和声音模式产生了巨大的感知敏感性。这种敏感性使我们能够在更大的人群中协调我们当下的行为与大规模的行为模式,并将我们自己的行为与那些大规模的模式进行比较。因此,基于互动性的方法所做出的最重要贡献是,它充分说明了人口水平的行为模式在控制短期尺度、此时此地的语言行为中所起的作用。在这样做的过程中,它为丰富的语言意义的生态解释提供了依据。
{"title":"Ecological meaning, linguistic meaning, and interactivity","authors":"S. Steffensen, M. Harvey","doi":"10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/COGSEM-2018-0005","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Human language is extraordinarily meaningful. Well-spoken or well-written passages can evoke our deepest emotions and elicit all manner of conscious and subconscious reactions. This is usually taken to be an insurmountable explanatory challenge for ecological approaches to cognitive science, the primary tools of which concern coordination dynamics in organism-environment systems. Recent work (Pattee, H. H. & J. Rączaszek-Leonardi 2012. Laws, Language, and Life. Dordrecht: Springer) has made headway in describing the meaningfulness of linguistic units — the kind of meaning that we perceive as mediated by specific symbols — within an ecological framework, by building an account based on Howard Pattee’s conceptualization of symbols as physical, replicable, historically-selected constraints on the dynamics of self-organizing systems (Pattee, H. H. 1969. How does a molecule become a message?. Developmental Biology 3(supplemental). 1016; Pattee, H. H. 1972. Laws and constraints, symbols and languages. In C. H. Waddington (ed.), Towards a Theoretical Biology, 248–258. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press). In order to propose an “interactivity-based” approach to linguistic meaning, this paper takes the following steps: first, it rejects the view of linguistic meaning as fully independent from organism-environment interactions, as exemplified by formal approaches in philosophical semantics. Second, it presents a cutting-edge example of an ecological approach to symbols, namely Joanna Rączaszek-Leonardi’s (Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2009. Symbols as constraints: The structuring role of dynamics and self-organization in natural language. Pragmatics and Cognition 17(3). 653–676. DOI:10.1075/pandc.17.3.09ras; Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. 2016. How does a word become a message? An illustration on a developmental time-scale. New Ideas in Psychology 42, Supplement C: 46–55. DOI:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.08.001) version of Pattee’s symbols-as-constraints model. Third, it reviews and critiques a recent attempt (Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., I. Nomikou, K. J. Rohlfing & T. W. Deacon. 2018. Language development from an ecological perspective: Ecologically valid ways to abstract symbols. Ecological Psychology 30(1). 39–73) to integrate the symbols-as-constraints model with Terrence Deacon, T. W. 1997. The Symbolic Species. New York: W. W. Norton and Company; Deacon, T. W. 2011. The symbol concept. In M. Tallerman & K. R. Gibson (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, 393–405. Oxford: Oxford University Press) semiotic view of symbols, arguing that the properties ascribed to linguistic symbols, both by Deacon and very widely throughout the cognitive sciences, are not properties of individual instances of linguistic action. Rather, they belong to a particular mode of description that draws generalizations across the phenomenological experience of many language users. Finally, it lays out the core components of a novel “interactivity-based” approach to linguistic","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89760231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
The role of diagram materiality in mathematics 图的物质性在数学中的作用
Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-03-26 DOI: 10.1515/cogsem-2016-0008
A. Steensen, M. W. Johansen
Abstract Based on semiotic analyses of examples from the history of mathematics, we claim that the influence of the material aspects of diagram tokens is anything but trivial. We offer an interpretation of examples of diagrammatic reasoning processes in mathematics according to which the mathematical ideas, arguments, and concepts in question are shaped by the physical features of the chosen diagram tokens.
摘要:基于对数学历史中例子的符号学分析,我们认为图符号的物质方面的影响绝不是微不足道的。我们提供了数学中图解推理过程的例子解释,根据这些例子,数学思想、论证和问题中的概念是由所选择的图解符号的物理特征塑造的。
{"title":"The role of diagram materiality in mathematics","authors":"A. Steensen, M. W. Johansen","doi":"10.1515/cogsem-2016-0008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2016-0008","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Based on semiotic analyses of examples from the history of mathematics, we claim that the influence of the material aspects of diagram tokens is anything but trivial. We offer an interpretation of examples of diagrammatic reasoning processes in mathematics according to which the mathematical ideas, arguments, and concepts in question are shaped by the physical features of the chosen diagram tokens.","PeriodicalId":52385,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Semiotics","volume":"1 1","pages":"183 - 201"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89961290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Cognitive Semiotics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1