首页 > 最新文献

Evolution and Human Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Modeling mate choice in a small-scale community: Applying couple simulation in the U.S. and Conambo, Ecuador 模拟小规模社区的配偶选择:在美国和厄瓜多尔科南博应用夫妻模拟法
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.007
Daniel Conroy-Beam , John Q. Patton , Cari D. Goetz , Aaron W. Lukaszewski , Brenda Bowser

The near totality of human mate choice research occurs in large-scale, urban, industrial populations. It is unclear to what extent lessons learned from such populations reflect generalizable features of human mating psychology as opposed to localized responses to the demands of these historically unusual environments. Here, we use couple simulation, an agent-based modeling technique, to compare models of mate choice across both a U.S. sample (n = 1678) and a sample of k = 15 couples from Conambo, Ecuador—a relatively remote community of horticultural-foragers in the Ecuadorian Amazon. The Conambo sample provides a unique opportunity to evaluate models of mate choice in that (1) this sample represents approximately 50% of the households within this community and (2) all of the participants in this sample are acquainted with one another. Participants in Conambo completed a ranking task in which each participant ranked each opposite-sex adult in the community in terms of their quality as a spouse. We used these rankings to simulate the mating market in Conambo under alternative models of mate choice. We find that these models are able to reproduce Conambo marriages at a high degree of accuracy and perform comparably across both the Conambo sample and U.S. samples. Specifically, the resource allocation model performs best in reproducing mate choices in both the U.S. and Conambo samples. These results suggest that at least some aspects of human mating psychology generalize across both large-scale industrialized and small-scale populations.

人类择偶研究几乎全部发生在大规模的城市工业人口中。目前还不清楚从这些人群中吸取的经验教训在多大程度上反映了人类交配心理的普遍特征,而不是对这些历史上不同寻常的环境需求的局部反应。在这里,我们使用夫妻模拟这种基于代理的建模技术,比较了美国样本(n = 1678)和厄瓜多尔科南博样本(k = 15 对夫妻)的择偶模型,科南博样本是厄瓜多尔亚马逊地区相对偏远的园艺狩猎者社区。科南博样本为评估择偶模型提供了一个独特的机会,因为:(1)该样本代表了该社区约 50% 的家庭;(2)该样本中的所有参与者都彼此熟悉。科南博的参与者完成了一项排名任务,每位参与者都对社区中的每位异性成年人进行了配偶质量排名。我们利用这些排名来模拟科南博交配市场的择偶模式。我们发现,这些模型能够高度准确地再现科南博的婚姻,并且在科南博样本和美国样本中的表现相当。具体而言,资源分配模型在再现美国和科南博样本中的择偶情况方面表现最佳。这些结果表明,人类交配心理的某些方面至少在大规模工业化人群和小规模人群中都具有普遍性。
{"title":"Modeling mate choice in a small-scale community: Applying couple simulation in the U.S. and Conambo, Ecuador","authors":"Daniel Conroy-Beam ,&nbsp;John Q. Patton ,&nbsp;Cari D. Goetz ,&nbsp;Aaron W. Lukaszewski ,&nbsp;Brenda Bowser","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.007","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.007","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The near totality of human mate choice research occurs in large-scale, urban, industrial populations. It is unclear to what extent lessons learned from such populations reflect generalizable features of human mating psychology as opposed to localized responses to the demands of these historically unusual environments. Here, we use couple simulation, an agent-based modeling technique, to compare models of mate choice across both a U.S. sample (<em>n</em> = 1678) and a sample of <em>k</em> = 15 couples from Conambo, Ecuador—a relatively remote community of horticultural-foragers in the Ecuadorian Amazon. The Conambo sample provides a unique opportunity to evaluate models of mate choice in that (1) this sample represents approximately 50% of the households within this community and (2) all of the participants in this sample are acquainted with one another. Participants in Conambo completed a ranking task in which each participant ranked each opposite-sex adult in the community in terms of their quality as a spouse. We used these rankings to simulate the mating market in Conambo under alternative models of mate choice. We find that these models are able to reproduce Conambo marriages at a high degree of accuracy and perform comparably across both the Conambo sample and U.S. samples. Specifically, the resource allocation model performs best in reproducing mate choices in both the U.S. and Conambo samples. These results suggest that at least some aspects of human mating psychology generalize across both large-scale industrialized and small-scale populations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 605-612"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134976786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural and contextual variation in first mover norms of ownership: evidence from an Achuar community 先行者所有权规范的文化和环境差异:阿丘阿尔社区的证据
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.004
Ulises J. Espinoza, H. Clark Barrett

The proposal that humans possess an evolved psychology of ownership is a highly plausible one. But what, if any, features of human ownership psychology might be universal? Psychologists have proposed that human ownership psychology might contain rules or norms for determining ownership, some of which might be universal. Here we explore first mover norms, in which an individual who acts first or exerts higher initial effort towards an object is recognized as its owner. Developmental studies in North America and Europe have provided evidence that first mover intuitions, especially about first possession, reliably develop in childhood, and some cross-cultural studies have supported this. Ethnographic research, however, provides mixed evidence about the universality of first-mover norms across cultures and domains. Here we report results from an experimental study comparing judgments of Achuar adults in Ecuador with those of an online U.S. sample. Achuar judgments leaned weakly in favor of first possessors in the domain of hunted game, but not for land. For land, a principle of use took precedence over first possession. U.S. participants, on the other hand, exhibited strong first possessor intuitions across both domains, consistent with prior results in the psychological literature. Together these findings suggest that first mover norms for assigning ownership may be more culturally and contextually variable than prior psychological research has suggested.

人类拥有一种进化而来的所有权心理,这一说法非常可信。但是,如果说人类的所有权心理具有普遍性的话,它又具有哪些普遍性呢?心理学家提出,人类的所有权心理可能包含确定所有权的规则或规范,其中一些可能具有普遍性。在此,我们将探讨先行者规范,在这种规范中,先行动或对物体付出较多初始努力的个体将被认定为其所有者。北美和欧洲的发展研究证明,先行者直觉,尤其是关于先行占有的直觉,是在儿童时期形成的,一些跨文化研究也支持这一观点。然而,人种学研究却提供了关于先行者规范在不同文化和领域的普遍性的不同证据。在此,我们报告了一项实验研究的结果,该研究比较了厄瓜多尔阿楚阿成人与美国在线样本的判断。在狩猎领域,阿丘亚尔人的判断微弱地倾向于第一占有者,但在土地领域则不然。在土地方面,使用原则优先于第一占有原则。另一方面,美国参与者在两个领域都表现出强烈的第一占有者直觉,这与之前心理学文献的结果一致。这些发现共同表明,分配所有权的先行者规范可能比之前的心理学研究表明的更具文化和背景差异。
{"title":"Cultural and contextual variation in first mover norms of ownership: evidence from an Achuar community","authors":"Ulises J. Espinoza,&nbsp;H. Clark Barrett","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The proposal that humans possess an evolved psychology of ownership is a highly plausible one. But what, if any, features of human ownership psychology might be universal? Psychologists have proposed that human ownership psychology might contain rules or norms for determining ownership, some of which might be universal. Here we explore <em>first mover</em> norms, in which an individual who acts first or exerts higher initial effort towards an object is recognized as its owner. Developmental studies in North America and Europe have provided evidence that first mover intuitions, especially about first possession, reliably develop in childhood, and some cross-cultural studies have supported this. Ethnographic research, however, provides mixed evidence about the universality of first-mover norms across cultures and domains. Here we report results from an experimental study comparing judgments of Achuar adults in Ecuador with those of an online U.S. sample. Achuar judgments leaned weakly in favor of first possessors in the domain of hunted game, but not for land. For land, a principle of use took precedence over first possession. U.S. participants, on the other hand, exhibited strong first possessor intuitions across both domains, consistent with prior results in the psychological literature. Together these findings suggest that first mover norms for assigning ownership may be more culturally and contextually variable than prior psychological research has suggested.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 584-596"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134917188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Editorial overview and dedication to John Patton: “Dispatches from the field: Insights from studies in ecologically diverse communities: Part 2” 编辑概述和对约翰·巴顿的奉献:“来自野外的报道:来自生态多样性社区研究的见解:第2部分”
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.005
Elizabeth G. Pillsworth , Aaron W. Lukaszewski
{"title":"Editorial overview and dedication to John Patton: “Dispatches from the field: Insights from studies in ecologically diverse communities: Part 2”","authors":"Elizabeth G. Pillsworth ,&nbsp;Aaron W. Lukaszewski","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.005","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 526-529"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138542148","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interpersonal conflicts and third-party mediation in a pastoralist society 牧民社会中的人际冲突和第三方调解
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.003
Zachary H. Garfield , Luke Glowacki

Human societies depend on the ability of their members to coordinate and cooperate with others. Yet, within-group conflict can threaten group stability. This threat is severe among humans due to the scale of our societies and the frequent low levels of relatedness between members. Our ability to resolve inter-individual conflicts is a key aspect of our species' success. Despite the importance of conflict resolution in human sociality, the socio-ecology of how within-group conflicts are resolved in naturalistic settings is underexplored. Using a sample of 160 inter-individual conflicts reported by 81 adults from an agro-pastoralist community in southwest Ethiopia, we identify the primary causes of interpersonal conflict and the features associated with third-party mediation and conflict outcomes. We find that both men and women experience relatively severe inter- and intra-gender conflicts; conflicts between women are more likely to be social in nature, while conflicts between men are more likely to be over resource control. Third-party mediation more often occurs in social conflicts rather than conflicts over material or subsistence resources and in conflicts between clan members and friends. Mediators in conflicts between women tend to be women while mediators for conflicts between men tend to be men. Women, however, are as equally likely as men to help mediate inter-gender conflicts, which suggests an important opportunity for female leadership in this patriarchal society. Although more than 80% of conflicts were resolved, social conflicts are more difficult to resolve than conflicts over resources. Conflict severity, clan membership, and the relationship between those in conflict are associated with severed relationships. These results underscore the importance of third-party mediators and inter-gender interactions in human societies and the importance of socio-economic structures in shaping interpersonal conflicts and their resolution.

人类社会依赖于其成员与他人协调合作的能力。然而,群体内部的冲突会威胁到群体的稳定。由于我们的社会规模庞大,而且成员之间的亲缘关系往往很低,因此这种威胁在人类中非常严重。我们解决个体间冲突的能力是我们这个物种成功的一个关键方面。尽管解决冲突在人类社会性中非常重要,但在自然环境中如何解决群体内部冲突的社会生态学研究却不足。我们利用埃塞俄比亚西南部一个农牧社区的 81 名成年人报告的 160 个个体间冲突样本,确定了人际冲突的主要原因以及与第三方调解和冲突结果相关的特征。我们发现,男性和女性都经历过相对严重的性别间和性别内冲突;女性之间的冲突更可能是社会性质的,而男性之间的冲突更可能是资源控制问题。第三方调解更多地发生在社会冲突中,而不是物质或生存资源冲突中,也更多地发生在氏族成员和朋友之间的冲突中。妇女之间冲突的调解人往往是妇女,而男子之间冲突的调解人往往是男子。不过,女性和男性一样有可能帮助调解性别间的冲突,这表明在这个父权制社会中,女性领导力是一个重要的机会。尽管超过 80% 的冲突得到了解决,但社会冲突比资源冲突更难解决。冲突的严重程度、氏族成员身份以及冲突双方之间的关系都与关系破裂有关。这些结果凸显了第三方调解人和性别间互动在人类社会中的重要性,以及社会经济结构在影响人际冲突及其解决方面的重要性。
{"title":"Interpersonal conflicts and third-party mediation in a pastoralist society","authors":"Zachary H. Garfield ,&nbsp;Luke Glowacki","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.003","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.10.003","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Human societies depend on the ability of their members to coordinate and cooperate with others. Yet, within-group conflict can threaten group stability. This threat is severe among humans due to the scale of our societies and the frequent low levels of relatedness between members. Our ability to resolve inter-individual conflicts is a key aspect of our species' success. Despite the importance of conflict resolution in human sociality, the socio-ecology of how within-group conflicts are resolved in naturalistic settings is underexplored. Using a sample of 160 inter-individual conflicts reported by 81 adults from an agro-pastoralist community in southwest Ethiopia, we identify the primary causes of interpersonal conflict and the features associated with third-party mediation and conflict outcomes. We find that both men and women experience relatively severe inter- and intra-gender conflicts; conflicts between women are more likely to be social in nature, while conflicts between men are more likely to be over resource control. Third-party mediation more often occurs in social conflicts rather than conflicts over material or subsistence resources and in conflicts between clan members and friends. Mediators in conflicts between women tend to be women while mediators for conflicts between men tend to be men. Women, however, are as equally likely as men to help mediate inter-gender conflicts, which suggests an important opportunity for female leadership in this patriarchal society. Although more than 80% of conflicts were resolved, social conflicts are more difficult to resolve than conflicts over resources. Conflict severity, clan membership, and the relationship between those in conflict are associated with severed relationships. These results underscore the importance of third-party mediators and inter-gender interactions in human societies and the importance of socio-economic structures in shaping interpersonal conflicts and their resolution.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 613-623"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135515897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Unmaking egalitarianism: Comparing sources of political change in an Amazonian society 破坏平均主义:比较亚马逊社会政治变革的来源
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.001
Christopher R. von Rueden

Politically egalitarian societies were likely more common in pre-history than in recent millenia. Why did societies become more hierarchical? Answers to this question remain debated, based on evidence largely drawn from archaeological case studies or comparison of societies from the ethnographic record. I suggest that modern small-scale societies transitioning to market economies can provide complementary tests of the sources of political inequality. I first describe moderate variation in men's influence during community meetings (i.e. political inequality) across four relatively egalitarian Tsimane villages in the Bolivian Amazon, as well as within one of these villages over twelve years. I then assess the roles of (1) sharing networks, (2) patron-client relationships, and (3) leadership opportunity in explaining that variation. Greater political inequality does not associate with reduced sharing but does associate with concentration of conflict mediation in the most influential men (per leadership opportunity) and more equivocally with intra-village paid labor (per patron-client models). In general, I argue that we need more micro-scale studies of societies in transition to understand why individuals come to tolerate greater political inequality.

政治上平等的社会在史前时期可能比近代更为常见。为什么社会变得更加等级森严?对这一问题的答案仍存在争议,主要依据的证据来自考古案例研究或人种学记录中的社会比较。我认为,向市场经济过渡的现代小规模社会可以对政治不平等的根源进行补充性检验。我首先描述了玻利维亚亚马逊地区四个相对平等的 Tsimane 村庄中男性在社区会议中影响力的适度变化(即政治不平等),以及其中一个村庄十二年来的情况。然后,我评估了(1)分享网络、(2)赞助人-客户关系和(3)领导机会在解释这种差异中的作用。更大的政治不平等与分享的减少无关,但与冲突调解集中于最有影响力的男性有关(根据领导机会),也与村内有偿劳动有关(根据赞助人-客户模式)。总之,我认为我们需要对转型社会进行更多的微观研究,以了解为什么个人会容忍更大的政治不平等。
{"title":"Unmaking egalitarianism: Comparing sources of political change in an Amazonian society","authors":"Christopher R. von Rueden","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Politically egalitarian societies were likely more common in pre-history than in recent millenia. Why did societies become more hierarchical? Answers to this question remain debated, based on evidence largely drawn from archaeological case studies or comparison of societies from the ethnographic record. I suggest that modern small-scale societies transitioning to market economies can provide complementary tests of the sources of political inequality. I first describe moderate variation in men's influence during community meetings (i.e. political inequality) across four relatively egalitarian Tsimane villages in the Bolivian Amazon, as well as within one of these villages over twelve years. I then assess the roles of (1) sharing networks, (2) patron-client relationships, and (3) leadership opportunity in explaining that variation. Greater political inequality does not associate with reduced sharing but does associate with concentration of conflict mediation in the most influential men (per leadership opportunity) and more equivocally with intra-village paid labor (per patron-client models). In general, I argue that we need more micro-scale studies of societies in transition to understand why individuals come to tolerate greater political inequality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 541-554"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48862230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Non-kin alloparents and child outcomes: Older siblings, but not godparents, predict educational attainment in a rural context 非亲缘关系的异父异母关系与儿童结果:在农村地区,年长的兄弟姐妹(而非干爹干妈)能预测受教育程度
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.006
Eric B. Hubbard , Ollie Shannon , Anne C. Pisor

Despite increasing evidence of non-kin contributions to cooperative childrearing, explicit investigation of non-kin allomaternal care remains rare, meaning we have little data to evaluate why non-kin provide care and whether non-kin investment translates into benefits for mothers or children. Here, we examine the role of godparents—kin and non-kin allomothers that often invest in both mothers and children—to investigate whether having a godparent translates into benefits in terms of the godchild's educational attainment. Among adults in two rural Bolivian communities, we find that having a godparent, whether kin or non-kin and regardless of where they live, does not predict years of education, finishing high school, or pursuing higher education. Instead, having more older siblings predicted educational attainment on all these dimensions. We make recommendations for how field researchers can better assess the impact of non-kin allomothers in future research.

尽管有越来越多的证据表明非亲缘关系对合作抚养子女做出了贡献,但对非亲缘关系异母照料的明确调查仍然很少见,这意味着我们几乎没有数据来评估非亲缘关系提供照料的原因,以及非亲缘关系的投资是否转化为母亲或子女的利益。在这里,我们研究了教父教母--通常对母亲和孩子都有投资的亲缘和非亲缘同母异父者--的作用,以调查拥有教父教母是否会转化为教子受教育程度方面的益处。在玻利维亚两个农村社区的成年人中,我们发现,无论教父教母是亲属还是非亲属,也无论他们住在哪里,拥有教父教母并不能预测受教育年限、完成高中学业或接受高等教育的情况。相反,拥有更多的年长兄弟姐妹却能预测所有这些方面的受教育程度。我们就实地研究人员如何在今后的研究中更好地评估非亲缘异母关系的影响提出了建议。
{"title":"Non-kin alloparents and child outcomes: Older siblings, but not godparents, predict educational attainment in a rural context","authors":"Eric B. Hubbard ,&nbsp;Ollie Shannon ,&nbsp;Anne C. Pisor","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.006","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.09.006","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite increasing evidence of non-kin contributions to cooperative childrearing, explicit investigation of non-kin allomaternal care remains rare, meaning we have little data to evaluate why non-kin provide care and whether non-kin investment translates into benefits for mothers or children. Here, we examine the role of godparents—kin and non-kin allomothers that often invest in both mothers and children—to investigate whether having a godparent translates into benefits in terms of the godchild's educational attainment. Among adults in two rural Bolivian communities, we find that having a godparent, whether kin or non-kin and regardless of where they live, does not predict years of education, finishing high school, or pursuing higher education. Instead, having more older siblings predicted educational attainment on all these dimensions. We make recommendations for how field researchers can better assess the impact of non-kin allomothers in future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 597-604"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136094571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Moralistic supernatural punishment is probably not associated with social complexity 道德上的超自然惩罚可能与社会复杂性无关
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.006
Aaron D. Lightner, Theiss Bendixen, Benjamin Grant Purzycki

Evolutionary theories of religion frequently assume that the presence of moralizing gods is positively associated with social complexity. An influential source of evidence for this assumption comes from researchers using the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample’s moralizing high gods variable as a proxy measure of their outcome of interest (the presence of moralizing gods). In this paper, we critically assess the common assumption that moralizing gods are associated with complex societies. We first discuss the high god variable’s coding criteria, which is defined by whether or not a god is the creator or director of the universe, regardless of power or omniscience. We then show that these criteria, which are not relevant to the question about whether gods are moralistic or punitive, has led researchers to underestimate the presence of moralizing gods by systematically producing false negatives – inferring that truly present moralizing gods are absent because moralizing high gods are absent. We then use datasets that include both moralizing gods and moralizing high gods to show that researchers risk inferring false negatives more frequently among lower levels of social complexity. As we also show, this likely leads to a spurious positive association between social complexity and the presence of moralizing gods. We then briefly discuss the ethnographic data and historical biases that might have strengthened this spurious association. We therefore question the widely assumed positive association between morally punitive gods and social complexity, and conclude that ethnographic evidence supports the prevalence of moralizing gods among small-scale societies.

宗教进化论经常假定,道德化神灵的存在与社会复杂性呈正相关。这一假设的一个有影响力的证据来源于研究人员使用标准跨文化样本中的道德化高神变量作为他们感兴趣的结果(道德化神灵的存在)的替代测量。在本文中,我们将对 "道德化神灵与复杂社会相关 "这一常见假设进行批判性评估。我们首先讨论了高位神变量的编码标准,其定义是神是否是宇宙的创造者或主宰者,而与权力或全知全能无关。然后我们表明,这些标准与神是否具有道德性或惩罚性的问题无关,它们导致研究人员低估了道德化神的存在,系统地产生了假阴性--推断出真正存在的道德化神不存在,因为道德化的高神不存在。然后,我们利用同时包含道德化神和道德化高神的数据集来说明,在社会复杂度较低的情况下,研究人员更有可能推断出错误的否定结果。我们还表明,这很可能导致社会复杂性与道德化神灵的存在之间出现虚假的正相关。然后,我们简要讨论了可能加强这种虚假联系的人种学数据和历史偏见。因此,我们质疑普遍认为的道德惩罚神与社会复杂性之间的正相关关系,并得出结论:人种学证据支持道德化神在小规模社会中的普遍存在。
{"title":"Moralistic supernatural punishment is probably not associated with social complexity","authors":"Aaron D. Lightner,&nbsp;Theiss Bendixen,&nbsp;Benjamin Grant Purzycki","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.006","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.006","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Evolutionary theories of religion frequently assume that <em>the presence of moralizing gods is positively associated with social complexity</em>. An influential source of evidence for this assumption comes from researchers using the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample’s moralizing <em>high gods</em> variable as a proxy measure of their outcome of interest (the presence of moralizing gods). In this paper, we critically assess the common assumption that moralizing gods are associated with complex societies. We first discuss the high god variable’s coding criteria, which is defined by whether or not a god is the creator or director of the universe, regardless of power or omniscience. We then show that these criteria, which are not relevant to the question about whether gods are moralistic or punitive, has led researchers to underestimate the presence of moralizing gods by systematically producing false negatives – inferring that truly present moralizing gods are absent because moralizing <em>high gods</em> are absent. We then use datasets that include both <em>moralizing gods</em> and <em>moralizing high gods</em> to show that researchers risk inferring false negatives more frequently among lower levels of social complexity. As we also show, this likely leads to a spurious positive association between social complexity and the presence of moralizing gods. We then briefly discuss the ethnographic data and historical biases that might have strengthened this spurious association. We therefore question the widely assumed positive association between morally punitive gods and social complexity, and conclude that ethnographic evidence supports the prevalence of moralizing gods among small-scale societies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Pages 555-565"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1090513822000812/pdfft?md5=298402ae8b18be8dacaa451ac3d7b3d4&pid=1-s2.0-S1090513822000812-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43715685","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Letter from the Editor 编辑来信
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.008
{"title":"Letter from the Editor","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.008","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.11.008","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 6","pages":"Page 525"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138509384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Friends near and afar, through thick and thin: Comparing contingency of help between close-distance and long-distance friends in Tanzanian fishing villages 远近朋友,风雨同舟:坦桑尼亚渔村远近朋友求助偶然性比较
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.004
Kristopher M. Smith , Anne C. Pisor , Bertha Aron , Kasambo Bernard , Paschal Fimbo , Rose Kimesera , Monique Borgerhoff Mulder

Humans form and maintain friendships across long distances, which can provide access to non-local resources and support against large shocks that affect the entire local community. However, there may be a greater risk of defection in long-distance friendships, as monitoring for defection is more difficult at greater distances; accordingly, help between long-distance friends may be more explicitly contingent than between close-distance friends. We interviewed 918 participants from 21 fishing villages in Tanzania about whether they had received help in the form of a gift or loan from a friend living in their village and a friend living in a neighboring village. As there are local expectations that loans will be repaid but gifts will not, we predicted that close-distance friends would be more likely to help with gifts, whereas long-distance friends would be more likely to help with loans. Contrary to our predictions, gifts and loans between close- and long-distance friends were similar in kind and amount, though close-distance friends provided help more frequently, possibly because close-distance friends are more likely to meet frequently and belong to the same religious congregation. These results indicate that long-distance friendships are an important, and likely robust, strategy for managing risk and accessing more resources.

人们形成并保持长距离的友谊,这可以提供获得非本地资源的途径,并支持应对影响整个当地社区的大冲击。然而,在远距离友谊中,背叛的风险可能更大,因为距离越远,监视背叛就越困难;因此,距离远的朋友之间的帮助可能比距离近的朋友之间的帮助更明显。我们采访了来自坦桑尼亚21个渔村的918名参与者,询问他们是否从住在他们村里的朋友和住在邻村的朋友那里得到过礼物或贷款的帮助。由于当地存在贷款会被偿还而礼物不会被偿还的预期,我们预测距离较近的朋友更有可能帮助送礼,而距离较远的朋友更有可能帮助贷款。与我们的预测相反,亲密朋友和异地朋友之间的礼物和贷款在种类和数量上是相似的,尽管距离近的朋友提供的帮助更频繁,可能是因为距离近的朋友更有可能经常见面,并且属于同一个宗教团体。这些结果表明,异地友谊是管理风险和获取更多资源的重要且可能是强有力的策略。
{"title":"Friends near and afar, through thick and thin: Comparing contingency of help between close-distance and long-distance friends in Tanzanian fishing villages","authors":"Kristopher M. Smith ,&nbsp;Anne C. Pisor ,&nbsp;Bertha Aron ,&nbsp;Kasambo Bernard ,&nbsp;Paschal Fimbo ,&nbsp;Rose Kimesera ,&nbsp;Monique Borgerhoff Mulder","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.09.004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Humans form and maintain friendships across long distances, which can provide access to non-local resources and support against large shocks that affect the entire local community. However, there may be a greater risk of defection in long-distance friendships, as monitoring for defection is more difficult at greater distances; accordingly, help between long-distance friends may be more explicitly contingent than between close-distance friends. We interviewed 918 participants from 21 fishing villages in Tanzania about whether they had received help in the form of a gift or loan from a friend living in their village and a friend living in a neighboring village. As there are local expectations that loans will be repaid but gifts will not, we predicted that close-distance friends would be more likely to help with gifts, whereas long-distance friends would be more likely to help with loans. Contrary to our predictions, gifts and loans between close- and long-distance friends were similar in kind and amount, though close-distance friends provided help more frequently, possibly because close-distance friends are more likely to meet frequently and belong to the same religious congregation. These results indicate that long-distance friendships are an important, and likely robust, strategy for managing risk and accessing more resources.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 5","pages":"Pages 454-465"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48790016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Punitive justice serves to restore reciprocal cooperation in three small-scale societies 惩罚性司法有助于在三个小规模社会中恢复互惠合作
IF 5.1 1区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.03.001
Léo Fitouchi , Manvir Singh

Fines, corporal punishments, and other procedures of punitive justice recur across small-scale societies. Although they are often assumed to enforce group norms, we here propose the relation-restoration hypothesis of punitive justice, according to which punitive procedures function to restore dyadic cooperation and curtail conflict between offender and victim following violations of reciprocal obligations. We test this hypothesis's predictions using observations of justice systems in three small-scale societies. We code ethnographic reports of 97 transgressions among Kiowa equestrian foragers (North America); analyze a sample of 302 transgressions among Mentawai horticulturalists (Indonesia); and review retributive procedures documented among Nuer pastoralists (South Sudan). Consistent with the relation-restoration hypothesis, we find that third-party punishment is rare; that most third-party involvement aims at resolving conflicts; that costs paid by offenders serve to achieve forgiveness by repairing victims; that punitive justice is accompanied by ceremonial procedures aimed at limiting conflict and restoring goodwill; and that failures to impose costs contribute to a decline in reciprocal cooperation. Although we document rare instances of third-party punishment among the Kiowa (6.6% of offenses), punitive justice more often serves as restorative justice, appeasing victims' urge for revenge while not overly harming offenders' interests to ensure reconciliation.

罚款、体罚和其他惩罚性司法程序在小规模社会中反复出现。虽然它们通常被认为是执行群体规范,但我们在此提出惩罚性司法的关系恢复假设,根据该假设,惩罚性程序的功能是恢复二元合作,并减少违反互惠义务后罪犯和受害者之间的冲突。我们通过对三个小规模社会的司法系统的观察来检验这一假设的预测。我们编码了97例基奥瓦骑马人(北美)越界行为的人种学报告;分析明打威园艺师(印度尼西亚)302例违规行为的样本;并审查努尔族牧民(南苏丹)记录在案的报复程序。与关系恢复假说一致,我们发现第三方惩罚是罕见的;大多数第三方参与的目的是解决冲突;罪犯付出的代价是为了通过修复受害者来获得宽恕;惩罚性司法伴随着旨在限制冲突和恢复善意的礼仪程序;未能施加成本会导致互惠合作的减少。虽然我们记录了基奥瓦人中很少有第三方惩罚的例子(占犯罪的6.6%),但惩罚性司法更多的是作为恢复性司法,安抚受害者复仇的冲动,同时不会过度损害犯罪者的利益,以确保和解。
{"title":"Punitive justice serves to restore reciprocal cooperation in three small-scale societies","authors":"Léo Fitouchi ,&nbsp;Manvir Singh","doi":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.03.001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.03.001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Fines, corporal punishments<span>, and other procedures of punitive justice<span> recur across small-scale societies. Although they are often assumed to enforce group norms, we here propose the relation-restoration hypothesis of punitive justice, according to which punitive procedures function to restore dyadic cooperation and curtail conflict between offender and victim following violations of reciprocal obligations. We test this hypothesis's predictions using observations of justice systems in three small-scale societies. We code ethnographic reports of 97 transgressions among Kiowa equestrian foragers (North America); analyze a sample of 302 transgressions among Mentawai horticulturalists (Indonesia); and review retributive procedures documented among Nuer pastoralists (South Sudan). Consistent with the relation-restoration hypothesis, we find that third-party punishment is rare; that most third-party involvement aims at resolving conflicts; that costs paid by offenders serve to achieve forgiveness by repairing victims; that punitive justice is accompanied by ceremonial procedures aimed at limiting conflict and restoring goodwill; and that failures to impose costs contribute to a decline in reciprocal cooperation. Although we document rare instances of third-party punishment among the Kiowa (6.6% of offenses), punitive justice more often serves as restorative justice, appeasing victims' urge for revenge while not overly harming offenders' interests to ensure reconciliation.</span></span></p></div>","PeriodicalId":55159,"journal":{"name":"Evolution and Human Behavior","volume":"44 5","pages":"Pages 502-514"},"PeriodicalIF":5.1,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43130156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Evolution and Human Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1