首页 > 最新文献

Behavioural Processes最新文献

英文 中文
Rhesus monkeys show greater habituation to repeated computer-generated images than do orangutans 与猩猩相比,恒河猴对重复的计算机生成图像表现出更强的习惯性。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105011
Mackenzie F. Webster , Jonathan W.M. Engelberg , Robert R. Hampton

Humans and several other species of animals have demonstrated the ability to use familiarity to recognize that they have seen images before. In prior experiments, orangutans failed to show use of familiarity in memory tasks, even when other solutions were not available. We tested for evidence of habituation, a decreased response to repeated stimuli, as a behavioral indicator that repeated images were familiar to subjects. Monkeys and orangutans selected the smallest target out of four while computerized images were presented as distractors. Latency to complete the target-finding task was compared between conditions in which the distractor image was a familiar, repeating image, a novel, never-before-seen image, or no distractor was present. Rhesus macaques showed significant habituation, and significantly more habituation than orangutans, in each of four experiments. Orangutans showed statistically reliable habituation in only one of the four experiments. These results are consistent with previous research in which orangutans failed to demonstrate familiarity. Because we expect that familiarity and habituation are evolutionarily ancient memory processes, we struggle to explain these surprising, but consistent findings. Future research is needed to determine why orangutans respond to computerized images in this peculiar way.

人类和其他一些动物物种已经证明,它们能够利用熟悉感来识别自己曾经见过的图像。在之前的实验中,猩猩在记忆任务中未能表现出使用熟悉感,即使在没有其他解决方案的情况下也是如此。我们测试了 "习惯化"(对重复刺激的反应减弱)的证据,作为重复图像被试熟悉的行为指标。当电脑图像作为干扰物出现时,猴子和猩猩会从四个目标中选择最小的目标。在分散注意力的图像是熟悉的重复图像、新奇的从未见过的图像或没有分散注意力的图像的条件下,比较了完成寻找目标任务的延迟时间。在四项实验中,猕猴均表现出明显的习惯化,而且习惯化程度明显高于猩猩。猩猩只在四个实验中的一个实验中表现出统计上可靠的习惯化。这些结果与之前的研究结果一致,即猩猩未能表现出熟悉性。由于我们预计熟悉和习惯化是进化过程中的古老记忆过程,因此我们很难解释这些令人惊讶但却一致的发现。未来的研究需要确定为什么猩猩会以这种奇特的方式对电脑图像做出反应。
{"title":"Rhesus monkeys show greater habituation to repeated computer-generated images than do orangutans","authors":"Mackenzie F. Webster ,&nbsp;Jonathan W.M. Engelberg ,&nbsp;Robert R. Hampton","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105011","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105011","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Humans and several other species of animals have demonstrated the ability to use familiarity to recognize that they have seen images before. In prior experiments, orangutans failed to show use of familiarity in memory tasks, even when other solutions were not available. We tested for evidence of habituation, a decreased response to repeated stimuli, as a behavioral indicator that repeated images were familiar to subjects. Monkeys and orangutans selected the smallest target out of four while computerized images were presented as distractors. Latency to complete the target-finding task was compared between conditions in which the distractor image was a familiar, repeating image, a novel, never-before-seen image, or no distractor was present. Rhesus macaques showed significant habituation, and significantly more habituation than orangutans, in each of four experiments. Orangutans showed statistically reliable habituation in only one of the four experiments. These results are consistent with previous research in which orangutans failed to demonstrate familiarity. Because we expect that familiarity and habituation are evolutionarily ancient memory processes, we struggle to explain these surprising, but consistent findings. Future research is needed to determine why orangutans respond to computerized images in this peculiar way.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139989188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spontaneous tempo production in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) and jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) 鹦鹉(Nymphicus hollandicus)和丛林乌鸦(Corvus macrorhyncos)的自发节奏生成
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007
Mathilde Le Covec , Dalila Bovet , Shigeru Watanabe , Ei-Ichi Izawa , Anne Bobin-Bègue

Musical and rhythmical abilities are poorly documented in non-human animals. Most of the existing studies focused on synchronisation performances to external rhythms. In humans, studies demonstrated that rhythmical processing (e. g. rhythm discrimination or synchronisation to external rhythm) is dependent of an individual measure: the individual tempo. It is assessed by asking participants to produce an endogenous isochronous rhythm (known as spontaneous motor tempo) without any specific instructions nor temporal cue. In non-human animal literature, studies describing spontaneous and endogenous production of motor tempo without any temporal clue are rare. This exploratory study aims to describe and compare the spontaneous motor tempo of cockatiels and jungle crows. Data were collected on spontaneous beak drumming behaviours of birds housed in laboratory. Inter beak strokes intervals were calculated from sound tracks of videos. The analyses revealed that inter beak strokes intervals are non-randomly distributed intervals and are isochronous. Recorded spontaneous motor tempos are significantly different among some cockatiels. Since we could only conduct statistical analysis with one corvid, we cannot conclude about this species. Our results suggest that cockatiels and jungle crows have individual tempos, thus encouraging further investigations.

关于非人类动物的音乐和节奏能力的文献很少。现有的研究大多集中于外部节奏的同步表现。对人类的研究表明,节奏处理(如节奏辨别或与外部节奏同步)取决于个体措施:个体节奏。其评估方法是要求参与者在没有任何特定指令或时间线索的情况下产生内源性等时节奏(即自发运动节奏)。在非人类动物的文献中,描述在没有任何时间线索的情况下自发产生内源性运动节奏的研究并不多见。本探索性研究旨在描述和比较凤头鹦鹉和丛林乌鸦的自发运动节奏。研究收集了实验室饲养的鸟类自发鼓喙行为的数据。根据视频的声轨计算喙间击鼓的时间间隔。分析结果表明,鸟类的喙击间隔是非随机分布的,并且是等时的。记录到的自发运动节奏在某些凤头鹦鹉中存在显著差异。由于我们只能对一种鸟类进行统计分析,因此无法对该物种得出结论。我们的研究结果表明,凤头鸦和丛林乌鸦有各自的节奏,因此值得进一步研究。
{"title":"Spontaneous tempo production in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) and jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos)","authors":"Mathilde Le Covec ,&nbsp;Dalila Bovet ,&nbsp;Shigeru Watanabe ,&nbsp;Ei-Ichi Izawa ,&nbsp;Anne Bobin-Bègue","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Musical and rhythmical abilities are poorly documented in non-human animals. Most of the existing studies focused on synchronisation performances to external rhythms. In humans, studies demonstrated that rhythmical processing (e. g. rhythm discrimination or synchronisation to external rhythm) is dependent of an individual measure: the individual tempo. It is assessed by asking participants to produce an endogenous isochronous rhythm (known as spontaneous motor tempo) without any specific instructions nor temporal cue. In non-human animal literature, studies describing spontaneous and endogenous production of motor tempo without any temporal clue are rare. This exploratory study aims to describe and compare the spontaneous motor tempo of cockatiels and jungle crows. Data were collected on spontaneous beak drumming behaviours of birds housed in laboratory. Inter beak strokes intervals were calculated from sound tracks of videos. The analyses revealed that inter beak strokes intervals are non-randomly distributed intervals and are isochronous. Recorded spontaneous motor tempos are significantly different among some cockatiels. Since we could only conduct statistical analysis with one corvid, we cannot conclude about this species. Our results suggest that cockatiels and jungle crows have individual tempos, thus encouraging further investigations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000226/pdfft?md5=09fff76501e3b12998a761fc4cb2f4c0&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000226-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139773808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spontaneous tempo production in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) and jungle crows (Corvus macrorhyncos) 鹦鹉(Nymphicus hollandicus)和丛林乌鸦(Corvus macrorhyncos)的自发节奏生成
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007
Mathilde Le Covec, Dalila Bovet, Shigeru Watanabe, E. Izawa, Anne Bobin-Bègue
{"title":"Spontaneous tempo production in cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) and jungle crows (Corvus macrorhyncos)","authors":"Mathilde Le Covec, Dalila Bovet, Shigeru Watanabe, E. Izawa, Anne Bobin-Bègue","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105007","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139833348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sample-comparison mapping and joint stimulus control 样本比较映射和联合刺激控制
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006
Carlos Pinto, José Miguel Castanheira Dinis
{"title":"Sample-comparison mapping and joint stimulus control","authors":"Carlos Pinto, José Miguel Castanheira Dinis","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139833719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sample-comparison mapping and joint stimulus control 样本比较映射和联合刺激控制
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006
Carlos Pinto, José Miguel Castanheira-Dinis

A matching-to-sample task with a 3-sample, 2-comparison mapping has been found to engender joint control by the stimuli that signaled the samples (keylight) and the inter-trial interval (houselight), with a trade-off in the degree of control exerted by each stimulus. To learn about the boundary conditions for the establishment of that joint stimulus control, we trained pigeons in a similar task, but featuring a one-to-one sample-comparison mapping, with two samples and two comparisons. To assess their relative influences, we ran two tests where each of the stimuli was removed: in one test, no sample keylight was presented, and in the other, the ITI was spent in darkness. Results were consistent with both stimuli influencing choice but there was no clear evidence of a trade-off between them. These results suggest that sample-comparison mapping and/or total number of samples may influence how an animal directs its attention to the stimuli available.

一项具有 3 个样本、2 个比较映射的从样本到样本的匹配任务被发现会引起样本信号刺激(keylight)和试验间歇刺激(houseelight)的联合控制,而每种刺激所施加的控制程度是有权衡的。为了了解建立这种联合刺激控制的边界条件,我们对鸽子进行了类似的训练,但采用的是一对一的样本-比较映射,即两个样本和两个比较。为了评估这两种刺激的相对影响,我们进行了两次测试,在这两次测试中,每种刺激都被移除:在一次测试中,没有出现样本关键光,而在另一次测试中,ITI 在黑暗中度过。结果表明,两种刺激都会影响选择,但没有明显证据表明两者之间存在权衡。这些结果表明,样本比较映射和/或样本总数可能会影响动物如何将其注意力引导到可用的刺激物上。
{"title":"Sample-comparison mapping and joint stimulus control","authors":"Carlos Pinto,&nbsp;José Miguel Castanheira-Dinis","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105006","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A matching-to-sample task with a 3-sample, 2-comparison mapping has been found to engender joint control by the stimuli that signaled the samples (keylight) and the inter-trial interval (houselight), with a trade-off in the degree of control exerted by each stimulus. To learn about the boundary conditions for the establishment of that joint stimulus control, we trained pigeons in a similar task, but featuring a one-to-one sample-comparison mapping, with two samples and two comparisons. To assess their relative influences, we ran two tests where each of the stimuli was removed: in one test, no sample keylight was presented, and in the other, the ITI was spent in darkness. Results were consistent with both stimuli influencing choice but there was no clear evidence of a trade-off between them. These results suggest that sample-comparison mapping and/or total number of samples may influence how an animal directs its attention to the stimuli available.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000214/pdfft?md5=7b376f9a45b0390a1dd2867c2536812d&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000214-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139774002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examination of the joint Simon effect in rats: Changes in task performance based on actions of the partner 研究大鼠的联合西蒙效应:根据同伴的行为改变任务表现
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105005
Noriko Katsu , Kazuo Okanoya

Nonhuman animals have demonstrated various cooperative behaviors; however, many examples can be interpreted as individual contributions to a task rather than true behavioral coordination. In this study, we used the joint Simon task in rats to determine whether the presence of and task sharing with a partner affected performance in a joint activity. Rats were trained to discriminate between two auditory stimuli (3 and 12 kHz tones) and individually performed an auditory Simon task. They were paired with another rat and tested to perform half of the task, while the other rat performed the other half (joint task condition). The Simon effect was confirmed when the two rats completed half of a joint task. In contrast, when they were placed side by side but only one rat completed half of the task, the Simon effect was not observed. Further analyses revealed that the Simon effect observed in the joint task could not be explained by the simple addition of the two half tasks. In conclusion, task sharing affected individual performance in rats.

非人类动物表现出了各种合作行为;然而,许多例子可以被解释为对任务的个体贡献,而不是真正的行为协调。在这项研究中,我们利用大鼠的联合西蒙任务来确定同伴的存在和任务分担是否会影响大鼠在联合活动中的表现。我们训练大鼠分辨两种听觉刺激(3 千赫兹和 12 千赫兹的音调),并让它们单独完成听觉西蒙任务。大鼠与另一只大鼠配对,测试它们完成一半任务,另一只大鼠完成另一半任务(联合任务条件)。当两只大鼠共同完成一半任务时,西蒙效应得到了证实。相反,当两只老鼠并排但只有一只老鼠完成一半任务时,西蒙效应没有被观察到。进一步的分析表明,在联合任务中观察到的西蒙效应不能用简单地增加两个半任务来解释。总之,任务分担会影响大鼠的个体表现。
{"title":"Examination of the joint Simon effect in rats: Changes in task performance based on actions of the partner","authors":"Noriko Katsu ,&nbsp;Kazuo Okanoya","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105005","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Nonhuman animals have demonstrated various cooperative behaviors; however, many examples can be interpreted as individual contributions to a task rather than true behavioral coordination. In this study, we used the joint Simon task in rats to determine whether the presence of and task sharing with a partner affected performance in a joint activity. Rats were trained to discriminate between two auditory stimuli (3 and 12 kHz tones) and individually performed an auditory Simon task. They were paired with another rat and tested to perform half of the task, while the other rat performed the other half (joint task condition). The Simon effect was confirmed when the two rats completed half of a joint task. In contrast, when they were placed side by side but only one rat completed half of the task, the Simon effect was not observed. Further analyses revealed that the Simon effect observed in the joint task could not be explained by the simple addition of the two half tasks. In conclusion, task sharing affected individual performance in rats.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139745958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Higher offspring mortality in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii) that spontaneously present with large nest building behaviour 自发出现大量筑巢行为的鹿鼠(Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii)后代死亡率较高。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-13 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105004
Heike Stoppel , Brian H. Harvey , De Wet Wolmarans

Nesting is a normal, evolutionary conserved rodent behavioural phenotype that is expressed for purposes of breeding, safety, and thermal regulation. Further, nesting is commonly assessed as marker of overall rodent health and wellbeing, with poorer nesting performance generally proposed to resemble a worse state of health. Deer mice can be bidirectionally separated with 30 % of mice presenting with excessively large nesting behaviour (LNB). All laboratory-housed deer mice are exposed to identical environmental conditions. Thus, the functional purpose of LNB remains unknown. Considering the evolutionary functions of nesting, we hypothesized that LNB will be related to an inflated drive to breed and nurse offspring. After breeding two generations of offspring from six ‘normal’ nesting (NNB) and seven LNB expressing pairs, our data showed that while as fertile as NNB expressing pairs, offspring survival of LNB mice were notably worse (67.9 % vs. 98.3 %). In conclusion, variance in nesting behaviour should be considered when animal health and wellbeing is considered, since it may point to underlying biobehavioural perturbations.

筑巢是一种正常的、进化保守的啮齿动物行为表型,其目的是为了繁殖、安全和热调节。此外,筑巢通常被评估为啮齿动物整体健康和福祉的标志,一般认为筑巢表现较差的啮齿动物健康状况较差。鹿鼠可能会出现双向分离,30%的鹿鼠会出现过大的筑巢行为(LNB)。所有实验室饲养的鹿小鼠都暴露在相同的环境条件下。因此,LNB 的功能性目的仍然未知。考虑到筑巢的进化功能,我们假设LNB与繁殖和哺育后代的驱动力膨胀有关。在对六对 "正常 "筑巢(NNB)小鼠和七对表达 LNB 的小鼠繁殖了两代后代后,我们的数据显示,虽然 LNB 小鼠与表达 NNB 的小鼠一样具有繁殖能力,但其后代的存活率却明显低于 NNB 小鼠(67.9% 对 98.3%)。总之,在考虑动物的健康和福利时,应该考虑筑巢行为的差异,因为它可能指向潜在的生物行为干扰。
{"title":"Higher offspring mortality in deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii) that spontaneously present with large nest building behaviour","authors":"Heike Stoppel ,&nbsp;Brian H. Harvey ,&nbsp;De Wet Wolmarans","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105004","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105004","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Nesting is a normal, evolutionary conserved rodent behavioural phenotype that is expressed for purposes of breeding, safety, and thermal regulation. Further, nesting is commonly assessed as marker of overall rodent health and wellbeing, with poorer nesting performance generally proposed to resemble a worse state of health. Deer mice can be bidirectionally separated with 30 % of mice presenting with excessively large nesting behaviour (LNB). All laboratory-housed deer mice are exposed to identical environmental conditions. Thus, the functional purpose of LNB remains unknown. Considering the evolutionary functions of nesting, we hypothesized that LNB will be related to an inflated drive to breed and nurse offspring. After breeding two generations of offspring from six ‘normal’ nesting (NNB) and seven LNB expressing pairs, our data showed that while as fertile as NNB expressing pairs, offspring survival of LNB mice were notably worse (67.9 % vs. 98.3 %). In conclusion, variance in nesting behaviour should be considered when animal health and wellbeing is considered, since it may point to underlying biobehavioural perturbations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0376635724000196/pdfft?md5=26ee33160b6eab4caf11f8754af742d4&pid=1-s2.0-S0376635724000196-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139740256","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why do predators attack parasitized prey? Insights from a probabilistic model and a literature survey 捕食者为何攻击被寄生的猎物?概率模型和文献调查的启示。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105002
Michal Segoli , Yves Papegay , Tamir Rozenberg , Eric Wajnberg

Predators and parasitoids often encounter parasitized prey or hosts during foraging. While the outcomes of such encounters have been extensively studied for insect parasitoids, the consequences of a predator encountering parasitized prey have received less attention. One extreme example involves the potter wasp Delta dimidiatipenne that frequently provision their nest with parasitized caterpillars, despite the low suitability of this prey for consumption by their offspring. This raises two main questions: (1) why do female potter wasps continue collecting parasitized caterpillars? and (2) is this an exceptional example, or do predatory insects often suffer from fitness costs due to encounters with parasitized prey? We addressed the first question using a probabilistic mathematical model predicting the value of discrimination between parasitized and unparasitized prey for the potter wasp, and the second question by surveying the literature for examples in which the parasitism status of prey affected prey susceptibility, suitability, or prey choice by a predator. The model demonstrates that only under certain conditions is discrimination against parasitized prey beneficial in terms of the potter wasp’s lifetime reproductive success. The literature survey suggests that the occurrence of encounters and consumption of parasitized prey is common, but the overall consequences of such interactions have rarely been quantified. We conclude that the profitability and ability of a predator to discriminate against parasitized prey under natural conditions may be limited and call for additional studies quantifying the outcome of such interactions.

捕食者和寄生虫在觅食过程中经常会遇到被寄生的猎物或寄主。昆虫寄生虫已经对这种相遇的结果进行了广泛研究,但捕食者遇到寄生猎物的后果却较少受到关注。一个极端的例子是陶蜂 Delta dimidiatipenne,它们经常为自己的巢穴提供被寄生的毛虫,尽管这种猎物不太适合它们的后代食用。这就提出了两个主要问题:(1)为什么雌性壶蜂继续收集被寄生的毛虫?我们利用一个概率数学模型预测了壶蜂区分寄生和未寄生猎物的价值,从而解决了第一个问题,并通过调查文献,找到了猎物的寄生状态影响捕食者对猎物的易感性、适宜性或猎物选择的实例,从而解决了第二个问题。该模型表明,只有在特定条件下,歧视被寄生的猎物才有利于壶蜂一生的繁殖成功。文献调查表明,遇到并吃掉寄生猎物的情况很常见,但这种相互作用的总体后果却很少被量化。我们的结论是,捕食者在自然条件下辨别寄生猎物的盈利能力和能力可能是有限的,并呼吁开展更多研究,量化此类互动的结果。
{"title":"Why do predators attack parasitized prey? Insights from a probabilistic model and a literature survey","authors":"Michal Segoli ,&nbsp;Yves Papegay ,&nbsp;Tamir Rozenberg ,&nbsp;Eric Wajnberg","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Predators and parasitoids often encounter parasitized prey or hosts during foraging. While the outcomes of such encounters have been extensively studied for insect parasitoids, the consequences of a predator encountering parasitized prey have received less attention. One extreme example involves the potter wasp <em>Delta dimidiatipenne</em> that frequently provision their nest with parasitized caterpillars, despite the low suitability of this prey for consumption by their offspring. This raises two main questions: (1) why do female potter wasps continue collecting parasitized caterpillars? and (2) is this an exceptional example, or do predatory insects often suffer from fitness costs due to encounters with parasitized prey? We addressed the first question using a probabilistic mathematical model predicting the value of discrimination between parasitized and unparasitized prey for the potter wasp, and the second question by surveying the literature for examples in which the parasitism status of prey affected prey susceptibility, suitability, or prey choice by a predator. The model demonstrates that only under certain conditions is discrimination against parasitized prey beneficial in terms of the potter wasp’s lifetime reproductive success. The literature survey suggests that the occurrence of encounters and consumption of parasitized prey is common, but the overall consequences of such interactions have rarely been quantified. We conclude that the profitability and ability of a predator to discriminate against parasitized prey under natural conditions may be limited and call for additional studies quantifying the outcome of such interactions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139711390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pica behavior of laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus domestica): Nauseated animals ingest kaolin, zeolite, bentonite, but not calcium carbonate chalk 实验鼠(Rattus norvegicus domestica)的呕吐行为:恶心的动物会摄入高岭土、沸石和膨润土,但不会摄入碳酸钙白垩。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105001
Sadahiko Nakajima

"Pica" refers to the ingestion of non-nutritive substances by animals that would not typically consume them. The pica behavior can be utilized to investigate the internal conditions of animals' bodies. For example, rats, due to neuroanatomical reasons, cannot vomit; nevertheless, when nauseated, they ingest kaolin clay. This renders the ingestion of kaolin a practical proxy for measuring nausea in rats. The question of whether rats consume minerals other than kaolin during nauseous episodes remains unanswered. This study aims to identify a mineral better suited for detecting nausea in rats. In two experiments, nausea was induced in laboratory rats by a single dose of lithium chloride (0.15 M, 2% bw), and their mineral consumption over the 24-hour period was measured. Experiment 1 compared three minerals between rat groups: kaolin sold for nausea detection (kaolin A), kaolin for ceramics (kaolin B), and zeolite. Nauseated rats consumed all minerals, with the highest consumption occurring with kaolin B. In Experiment 2, three commercially available health soils were compared: edible kaolin, edible bentonite, and edible chalk. The most significant consumption was observed in the kaolin group, followed by the bentonite group, while nauseated rats did not consume edible chalk. These findings underscore the suitability of kaolin for nausea detection, although the extent of consumption may vary depending on the product.

"皮卡 "指的是动物摄入非营养物质,而这些物质通常不会被动物食用。皮卡行为可以用来研究动物身体的内部状况。例如,由于神经解剖学的原因,大鼠不能呕吐,但当恶心时,它们会摄入高岭土。因此,摄入高岭土是测量大鼠恶心程度的实用替代物。至于大鼠在恶心发作时是否会摄入高岭土以外的矿物质,这个问题仍然没有答案。本研究旨在确定一种更适合检测大鼠恶心症状的矿物质。在两项实验中,通过单剂量氯化锂(0.15M,2% 体重)诱发实验鼠恶心,并测量它们在 24 小时内的矿物质消耗量。实验 1 比较了大鼠组之间的三种矿物质:用于检测恶心的高岭土(高岭土 A)、用于陶瓷的高岭土(高岭土 B)和沸石。实验 2 比较了三种市售的健康土壤:食用高岭土、食用膨润土和食用白垩。高岭土组的消耗量最大,其次是膨润土组,而恶心的大鼠没有消耗食用白垩。这些发现强调了高岭土在恶心检测中的适用性,尽管食用量可能因产品而异。(215个字)。
{"title":"Pica behavior of laboratory rats (Rattus norvegicus domestica): Nauseated animals ingest kaolin, zeolite, bentonite, but not calcium carbonate chalk","authors":"Sadahiko Nakajima","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105001","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.105001","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>\"Pica\" refers to the ingestion of non-nutritive substances by animals that would not typically consume them. The pica behavior can be utilized to investigate the internal conditions of animals' bodies. For example, rats, due to neuroanatomical reasons, cannot vomit; nevertheless, when nauseated, they ingest kaolin clay. This renders the ingestion of kaolin a practical proxy for measuring nausea in rats. The question of whether rats consume minerals other than kaolin during nauseous episodes remains unanswered. This study aims to identify a mineral better suited for detecting nausea in rats. In two experiments, nausea was induced in laboratory rats by a single dose of lithium chloride (0.15 M, 2% bw), and their mineral consumption over the 24-hour period was measured. Experiment 1 compared three minerals between rat groups: kaolin sold for nausea detection (kaolin A), kaolin for ceramics (kaolin B), and zeolite. Nauseated rats consumed all minerals, with the highest consumption occurring with kaolin B. In Experiment 2, three commercially available health soils were compared: edible kaolin, edible bentonite, and edible chalk. The most significant consumption was observed in the kaolin group, followed by the bentonite group, while nauseated rats did not consume edible chalk. These findings underscore the suitability of kaolin for nausea detection, although the extent of consumption may vary depending on the product.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139711388","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to measure exploration: A combined estimation method. 如何测量勘探:综合估算法。
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2024.104998
Rosanne Beukeboom , David Benhaïm

The personality axis ‘exploration-avoidance’ is often measured using an open field test (OFT) from which measurements such as mobility level (e.g. distance travelled) and space use (e.g. area covered) in an unknown environment are extracted. Recently developed tracking software surprisingly includes only a measure of mobility level in their output summary. Consequently, recent articles using these software programs started to ignore a measure of space use in their determination of exploration. In this short communication, we show briefly why it is important to not overlook a measure of space use, provide an easy way to calculate such a measure, with adjustable resolution, from the available position data provided by the software and a method to determine a proxy for exploration.

探索-回避 "性格轴的测量通常采用开放场地测试(OFT),从中提取未知环境中的移动水平(如行进距离)和空间使用(如覆盖面积)等测量值。最近开发的跟踪软件在其输出摘要中竟然只包括移动水平的测量。因此,最近一些使用这些软件程序的文章在确定勘探情况时开始忽略空间使用情况。在这篇简短的文章中,我们将简要说明为什么不能忽略空间利用的衡量标准,提供一种从软件提供的可用位置数据中计算这种衡量标准的简便方法(分辨率可调),以及一种确定探索替代值的方法。
{"title":"How to measure exploration: A combined estimation method.","authors":"Rosanne Beukeboom ,&nbsp;David Benhaïm","doi":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.104998","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.beproc.2024.104998","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The personality axis ‘exploration-avoidance’ is often measured using an open field test (OFT) from which measurements such as mobility level (e.g. distance travelled) and space use (e.g. area covered) in an unknown environment are extracted. Recently developed tracking software surprisingly includes only a measure of mobility level in their output summary. Consequently, recent articles using these software programs started to ignore a measure of space use in their determination of exploration. In this short communication, we show briefly why it is important to not overlook a measure of space use, provide an easy way to calculate such a measure, with adjustable resolution, from the available position data provided by the software and a method to determine a proxy for exploration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":8746,"journal":{"name":"Behavioural Processes","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139711387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavioural Processes
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1