首页 > 最新文献

International Review of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
High Economic Inequality Makes Us Feel Less Wealthy 高度的经济不平等使我们感觉不那么富裕
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-11-07 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.333
Ángel Sánchez‐Rodríguez, J. Jetten, G. Willis, Rosa Rodríguez-Bailón
Given that people evaluate their own income and that of their group by comparing it with that of others, economic inequality (i.e., the gap between the poor and the wealthy) may affect how people perceive their ingroup wealth. Additional analyses of data of six previously conducted studies (n = 747) provided initial evidence that individuals with average levels of affluence felt that their income group was less wealthy when inequality was high than when it was low. The seventh study consisted of a preregistered experiment to independently test our hypothesis (n = 222). As predicted, compared to participants in the low inequality condition, those in the high inequality condition perceived their own group as less wealthy. The discussion focuses on the importance of considering self-perceived wealth in research on economic inequality.
鉴于人们通过与他人的收入进行比较来评估自己和群体的收入,经济不平等(即穷人和富人之间的差距)可能会影响人们对其内部财富的看法。对先前进行的六项研究(n=747)的数据进行的额外分析提供了初步证据,表明具有平均富裕水平的个人认为,当不平等程度高时,他们的收入群体不如不平等程度低时富裕。第七项研究由一个预先注册的实验组成,以独立检验我们的假设(n=222)。正如预测的那样,与处于低不平等条件下的参与者相比,处于高不平等条件的参与者认为自己的群体不那么富有。讨论的重点是在经济不平等研究中考虑自我感知财富的重要性。
{"title":"High Economic Inequality Makes Us Feel Less Wealthy","authors":"Ángel Sánchez‐Rodríguez, J. Jetten, G. Willis, Rosa Rodríguez-Bailón","doi":"10.5334/irsp.333","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.333","url":null,"abstract":"Given that people evaluate their own income and that of their group by comparing it with that of others, economic inequality (i.e., the gap between the poor and the wealthy) may affect how people perceive their ingroup wealth. Additional analyses of data of six previously conducted studies (n = 747) provided initial evidence that individuals with average levels of affluence felt that their income group was less wealthy when inequality was high than when it was low. The seventh study consisted of a preregistered experiment to independently test our hypothesis (n = 222). As predicted, compared to participants in the low inequality condition, those in the high inequality condition perceived their own group as less wealthy. The discussion focuses on the importance of considering self-perceived wealth in research on economic inequality.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49075262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 23
The Face of Sexualization: Faces Wearing Makeup are Processed Less Configurally than Faces Without Makeup 性感化的脸:化妆后的脸比素颜的脸更少被处理
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-10-23 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.211
P. Bernard, Philippine Geelhand, L. Servais
Human bodies are sometimes cognitively objectified, i.e., processed less configurally and more analytically, in a way that resembles how most objects are perceived. Whereas how people process images of sexualized bodies appearing in the mass media has been well documented; whether subtler manifestations of sexualization, such as wearing makeup, might elicit cognitive objectification of ordinary women’s faces, remains unclear. The present paper aims at filling this gap. We hypothesized that faces wearing makeup would be processed less configurally than faces wearing no makeup. Sixty participants took part in a face recognition task, in which faces wearing or not wearing makeup were presented. In regards to faces with no makeup, people recognized face parts better in the context of whole faces than in isolation, which served as evidence of configural processing. In regards to faces wearing makeup, face parts were recognized equally well when presented in isolation vs. in the context of whole faces; evidence of a lower configural processing. That pattern of results was driven by eye makeup (vs. lipstick). Implications for research on objectification and sexualization are discussed.
人体有时在认知上被物化,即在结构上处理得更少,在分析上处理得更多,其方式类似于大多数物体的感知方式。然而,人们如何处理大众媒体上出现的性化身体的图像已经有了很好的记录;性化的微妙表现,如化妆,是否会引发普通女性面部的认知客体化,目前尚不清楚。本文件旨在填补这一空白。我们假设,化妆的脸会比不化妆的脸处理得更少。60名参与者参加了一项人脸识别任务,在该任务中,呈现化妆或未化妆的人脸。关于不化妆的脸,人们在整个脸的背景下比孤立地识别面部部位更好,这是构形处理的证据。关于化妆的脸,当单独呈现时与在整张脸的背景下呈现时,面部部位被同样好地识别;较低构形加工的证据。这种模式的结果是由眼妆(相对于口红)驱动的。讨论了对对象化和性化研究的启示。
{"title":"The Face of Sexualization: Faces Wearing Makeup are Processed Less Configurally than Faces Without Makeup","authors":"P. Bernard, Philippine Geelhand, L. Servais","doi":"10.5334/irsp.211","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.211","url":null,"abstract":"Human bodies are sometimes cognitively objectified, i.e., processed less configurally and more analytically, in a way that resembles how most objects are perceived. Whereas how people process images of sexualized bodies appearing in the mass media has been well documented; whether subtler manifestations of sexualization, such as wearing makeup, might elicit cognitive objectification of ordinary women’s faces, remains unclear. The present paper aims at filling this gap. We hypothesized that faces wearing makeup would be processed less configurally than faces wearing no makeup. Sixty participants took part in a face recognition task, in which faces wearing or not wearing makeup were presented. In regards to faces with no makeup, people recognized face parts better in the context of whole faces than in isolation, which served as evidence of configural processing. In regards to faces wearing makeup, face parts were recognized equally well when presented in isolation vs. in the context of whole faces; evidence of a lower configural processing. That pattern of results was driven by eye makeup (vs. lipstick). Implications for research on objectification and sexualization are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43684786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Collectivism and Individualism in Status Hierarchies: Socialization and Social Identity Explanations 地位等级中的集体主义与个人主义:社会化与社会认同解释
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-10-09 DOI: 10.5334/irsp.285
Vincenzo Iacoviello, F. Lorenzi‐Cioldi
Social scientists have produced major theoretical and empirical advances documenting the importance of group status in shaping the group members’ perceptions of themselves and of society at large. Evidence is accumulating showing that members of high-status groups favor individualistic and autonomous self-conceptions and worldviews, whereas members of low-status groups turn to more collectivistic and less personalized ones. This paper reports on research that has examined this phenomenon with a focus on social class divisions. It outlines two main explanations that have been developed to account for this self-group discrepancy in status hierarchies. One explanation rests on the long run diverging socialization processes that take place in high and low social classes. A complementary explanation is based on social identity dynamics. It suggests that such orientations result from differing motivations among members of high-status and low-status groups: While the former aim to protect a positive social identity, the latter must cope with a social identity threat.
社会科学家已经取得了重大的理论和实证进展,证明了群体地位在塑造群体成员对自己和整个社会的看法方面的重要性。越来越多的证据表明,高地位群体的成员倾向于个人主义和自主的自我概念和世界观,而低地位群体的成员则倾向于集体主义和不那么个性化的概念和世界观。本文报告了一项研究,该研究以社会阶级划分为重点,研究了这一现象。它概述了两种主要的解释,用来解释这种自我群体在地位层次上的差异。一种解释是,从长远来看,发生在社会上层和下层的社会化过程是不同的。一个补充的解释是基于社会身份动力学。研究表明,这种倾向是由高地位群体和低地位群体成员的不同动机造成的:前者旨在保护积极的社会身份,后者必须应对社会身份威胁。
{"title":"Collectivism and Individualism in Status Hierarchies: Socialization and Social Identity Explanations","authors":"Vincenzo Iacoviello, F. Lorenzi‐Cioldi","doi":"10.5334/irsp.285","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.285","url":null,"abstract":"Social scientists have produced major theoretical and empirical advances documenting the importance of group status in shaping the group members’ perceptions of themselves and of society at large. Evidence is accumulating showing that members of high-status groups favor individualistic and autonomous self-conceptions and worldviews, whereas members of low-status groups turn to more collectivistic and less personalized ones. This paper reports on research that has examined this phenomenon with a focus on social class divisions. It outlines two main explanations that have been developed to account for this self-group discrepancy in status hierarchies. One explanation rests on the long run diverging socialization processes that take place in high and low social classes. A complementary explanation is based on social identity dynamics. It suggests that such orientations result from differing motivations among members of high-status and low-status groups: While the former aim to protect a positive social identity, the latter must cope with a social identity threat.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-10-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46030468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Can Violent Video Game-Related Aggression Spread to Others? Effects on Retaliatory and Displaced Aggression 与电子游戏相关的暴力攻击会传播给他人吗?对报复性和流离失所侵略的影响
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-08-14 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.242
Martin Delhove, T. Greitemeyer
Exposure to violent video games (VVE) has been shown to increase aggression in the player. In the present two experiments, we tested whether this effect spreads within a social network. In Experiment 1, using data from a previous study that showed an effect of VVE on aggression in a Competitive Reaction Time Task (Greitemeyer, 2014), we assigned the blasts of noise selected by players to new participants. Afterwards, they selected these parameters themselves. VVE had a causal effect on retaliatory aggression, showing that the effect of VVE may spread on non-players. In Experiment 2, 200 participants played either a violent or nonviolent video game and participated in a Cold Water Task (CWT) in which they assigned the time another person has to keep the hand in painfully cold water. Two-hundred new participants were yoked to one of the video game participants and had to keep their hand in the water for the indicated duration. Afterwards, they assigned the time to the video game participant (retaliation) or to another person (displaced aggression). Although VVE had no significant impact, we found that higher CWT duration led participants to behave more aggressively not only when retaliating, but also, to a lesser extent, in the displaced aggression condition. Implications of the spreading effect of aggression in the context of VVE are discussed.
接触暴力电子游戏(VVE)已被证明会增加玩家的攻击性。在目前的两个实验中,我们测试了这种效应是否会在社会网络中传播。在实验1中,我们使用了之前一项研究的数据,该研究显示了VVE对竞争性反应时间任务中的攻击性的影响(Greitemeyer, 2014),我们将玩家选择的噪音爆炸分配给了新参与者。然后,他们自己选择这些参数。VVE对报复性攻击存在因果效应,表明VVE的影响可能会扩散到非玩家身上。在实验2中,200名参与者玩暴力或非暴力的电子游戏,并参与冷水任务(CWT)。在冷水任务中,他们指定另一个人将手放在痛苦的冷水中。200名新参与者与其中一名视频游戏参与者绑在一起,他们必须在指定的时间内把手放在水里。之后,他们将时间分配给视频游戏参与者(报复)或另一个人(流离失所的侵略)。虽然VVE没有显著影响,但我们发现,更长的CWT持续时间不仅会导致参与者在报复时表现出更强的攻击性,而且会在较小程度上导致参与者在迁移攻击条件下表现出更强的攻击性。本文讨论了攻击行为在虚拟虚拟情境下的传播效应。
{"title":"Can Violent Video Game-Related Aggression Spread to Others? Effects on Retaliatory and Displaced Aggression","authors":"Martin Delhove, T. Greitemeyer","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.242","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.242","url":null,"abstract":"Exposure to violent video games (VVE) has been shown to increase aggression in the player. In the present two experiments, we tested whether this effect spreads within a social network. In Experiment 1, using data from a previous study that showed an effect of VVE on aggression in a Competitive Reaction Time Task (Greitemeyer, 2014), we assigned the blasts of noise selected by players to new participants. Afterwards, they selected these parameters themselves. VVE had a causal effect on retaliatory aggression, showing that the effect of VVE may spread on non-players. In Experiment 2, 200 participants played either a violent or nonviolent video game and participated in a Cold Water Task (CWT) in which they assigned the time another person has to keep the hand in painfully cold water. Two-hundred new participants were yoked to one of the video game participants and had to keep their hand in the water for the indicated duration. Afterwards, they assigned the time to the video game participant (retaliation) or to another person (displaced aggression). Although VVE had no significant impact, we found that higher CWT duration led participants to behave more aggressively not only when retaliating, but also, to a lesser extent, in the displaced aggression condition. Implications of the spreading effect of aggression in the context of VVE are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48598319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Taking Parametric Assumptions Seriously: Arguments for the Use of Welch’s F-test instead of the Classical F-test in One-Way ANOVA 认真对待参数假设:在单因素方差分析中使用韦尔奇f检验而不是经典f检验的争论
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-08-01 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.198
Marie Delacre, C. Leys, Youri L. Mora, D. Lakens
Student’s t-test and classical F-test ANOVA rely on the assumptions that two or more samples are independent, and that independent and identically distributed residuals are normal and have equal variances between groups. We focus on the assumptions of normality and equality of variances, and argue that these assumptions are often unrealistic in the field of psychology. We underline the current lack of attention to these assumptions through an analysis of researchers’ practices. Through Monte Carlo simulations, we illustrate the consequences of performing the classic parametric F-test for ANOVA when the test assumptions are not met on the Type I error rate and statistical power. Under realistic deviations from the assumption of equal variances, the classic F-test can yield severely biased results and lead to invalid statistical inferences. We examine two common alternatives to the F-test, namely the Welch’s ANOVA (W-test) and the Brown-Forsythe test (F*-test). Our simulations show that under a range of realistic scenarios, the W-test is a better alternative and we therefore recommend using the W-test by default when comparing means. We provide a detailed example explaining how to perform the W-test in SPSS and R. We summarize our conclusions in practical recommendations that researchers can use to improve their statistical practices.
学生t检验和经典f检验ANOVA依赖于两个或多个样本是独立的假设,并且独立和同分布的残差是正态的,并且组间方差相等。我们关注正态性和方差相等的假设,并认为这些假设在心理学领域往往是不现实的。通过对研究人员实践的分析,我们强调目前缺乏对这些假设的关注。通过蒙特卡罗模拟,我们说明了在I型错误率和统计功率不满足检验假设时,对ANOVA进行经典参数f检验的后果。在实际偏离方差相等假设的情况下,经典的f检验会产生严重的偏倚结果,导致无效的统计推断。我们研究了F检验的两种常见替代方法,即韦尔奇方差分析(w检验)和布朗-福赛检验(F*检验)。我们的模拟表明,在一系列现实场景下,w检验是一个更好的选择,因此我们建议在比较均值时默认使用w检验。我们提供了一个详细的例子来解释如何在SPSS和r中执行w检验。我们将我们的结论总结为研究人员可以使用的实用建议,以改进他们的统计实践。
{"title":"Taking Parametric Assumptions Seriously: Arguments for the Use of Welch’s F-test instead of the Classical F-test in One-Way ANOVA","authors":"Marie Delacre, C. Leys, Youri L. Mora, D. Lakens","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.198","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.198","url":null,"abstract":"Student’s t-test and classical F-test ANOVA rely on the assumptions that two or more samples are independent, and that independent and identically distributed residuals are normal and have equal variances between groups. We focus on the assumptions of normality and equality of variances, and argue that these assumptions are often unrealistic in the field of psychology. We underline the current lack of attention to these assumptions through an analysis of researchers’ practices. Through Monte Carlo simulations, we illustrate the consequences of performing the classic parametric F-test for ANOVA when the test assumptions are not met on the Type I error rate and statistical power. Under realistic deviations from the assumption of equal variances, the classic F-test can yield severely biased results and lead to invalid statistical inferences. We examine two common alternatives to the F-test, namely the Welch’s ANOVA (W-test) and the Brown-Forsythe test (F*-test). Our simulations show that under a range of realistic scenarios, the W-test is a better alternative and we therefore recommend using the W-test by default when comparing means. We provide a detailed example explaining how to perform the W-test in SPSS and R. We summarize our conclusions in practical recommendations that researchers can use to improve their statistical practices.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47897145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 129
Architectural Forms of Collective Memory 集体记忆的建筑形式
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-07-05 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.236
Pascal Moliner, I. Bovina
In 2015, the Moscow City Hall and the Gulag History Museum launched a competition for the creation of a commemorative monument dedicated to the victims of political repression in Russia during the 1920s to the 1950s. In this article we present three studies carried out on an array of 309 images (digital images, model photographs, drawings) related to the projects submitted for this competition. Each project image was accompanied by a text written by the author (artist, architect, designer). These images and texts were analyzed within the framework of the concept of collective memory and the theory of social representations. The first study focused on the texts and suggested that there are two dimensions of the collective memory about the commemorated event, a historical dimension and a human dimension. The second study focused on images, two dominant forms in the monument projects were identified. The third study showed that depending on whether the accompanying text favors the historical dimension of the event or its human dimension, the designers did not make identical use of the different architectural forms (identified in the previous study) to elaborate their project. These results suggest that the architectural forms of monument projects vary according to the historical vs. human dimension favored by the collective memory of their authors.
2015年,莫斯科市政厅和古拉格历史博物馆发起了一项竞赛,旨在为俄罗斯20世纪20年代至50年代政治镇压的受害者建造一座纪念纪念碑。在这篇文章中,我们介绍了对309张与本次竞赛项目相关的图像(数字图像、模型照片、图纸)进行的三项研究。每个项目图片都附有作者(艺术家、建筑师、设计师)撰写的文字。这些图像和文本在集体记忆概念和社会表征理论的框架内进行了分析。第一项研究侧重于文本,并提出关于纪念事件的集体记忆有两个维度,一个是历史维度,一个是人的维度。第二个研究重点是图像,确定了纪念碑项目的两种主要形式。第三项研究表明,根据所附文本是倾向于事件的历史维度还是人文维度,设计师并没有完全使用不同的建筑形式(在前一项研究中确定)来阐述他们的项目。这些结果表明,纪念碑项目的建筑形式根据其作者的集体记忆所青睐的历史与人文维度而变化。
{"title":"Architectural Forms of Collective Memory","authors":"Pascal Moliner, I. Bovina","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.236","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.236","url":null,"abstract":"In 2015, the Moscow City Hall and the Gulag History Museum launched a competition for the creation of a commemorative monument dedicated to the victims of political repression in Russia during the 1920s to the 1950s. In this article we present three studies carried out on an array of 309 images (digital images, model photographs, drawings) related to the projects submitted for this competition. Each project image was accompanied by a text written by the author (artist, architect, designer). These images and texts were analyzed within the framework of the concept of collective memory and the theory of social representations. The first study focused on the texts and suggested that there are two dimensions of the collective memory about the commemorated event, a historical dimension and a human dimension. The second study focused on images, two dominant forms in the monument projects were identified. The third study showed that depending on whether the accompanying text favors the historical dimension of the event or its human dimension, the designers did not make identical use of the different architectural forms (identified in the previous study) to elaborate their project. These results suggest that the architectural forms of monument projects vary according to the historical vs. human dimension favored by the collective memory of their authors.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42056847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Expression of Concern Regarding Six Articles by Dr. Nicolas Guéguen Nicolas gusamuguen博士对六篇文章的关注表达
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-06-19 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.304
Céline Darnon, O. Klein
The editors of the International Review of Social Psychology are expressing concern regarding the reliability of data published in content in the journal relating to six papers authored by Nicolas Gueguen.
《国际社会心理学评论》的编辑们对该杂志上发表的与尼古拉斯·盖根撰写的六篇论文有关的数据的可靠性表示担忧。
{"title":"Expression of Concern Regarding Six Articles by Dr. Nicolas Guéguen","authors":"Céline Darnon, O. Klein","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.304","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.304","url":null,"abstract":"The editors of the International Review of Social Psychology are expressing concern regarding the reliability of data published in content in the journal relating to six papers authored by Nicolas Gueguen.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43492200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: La technique du « vous êtes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberté et soumission à une requête ou le paradoxe d’une liberté manipulatrice 纠正:“你从……中获得自由”的技巧:诱导一种自由的感觉,服从一个要求或操纵自由的悖论
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-06-19 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.303
A. Pascual
This article details a correction to the article: Pascual, A., & Gueguen, N. (2002). La technique du « vous etes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberte et soumission a une requete ou le paradoxe d’une liberte manipulatrice [The « You are free of… » technique: induction of a feeling of freedom and compliance in a request or the paradox of a manipulating freedom]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 15(1), 51–80. Resume Cet article est une correction de l’article : Pascual, A., & Gueguen, N. (2002). La technique du « vous etes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberte et soumission a une requete ou le paradoxe d’une liberte manipulatrice [The « You are free of… » technique: induction of a feeling of freedom and compliance in a request or the paradox of a manipulating freedom]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 15(1), 51–80.
这篇文章详细介绍了对这篇文章的修改:Pascual, a ., & Gueguen, N.(2002)。«...空闲的人你的技术»:感应的自由感,并且提交了一个请求或操纵性自由的悖论》[The«You are free of ...»:诱导技术顺应了歌of freedom and in a request黄金a manipulating The paradox of freedom)。国际社会心理学评论,15(1),51 - 80。本文是对Pascual, A., & Gueguen, N.(2002)的文章的修正。«...空闲的人你的技术»:感应的自由感,并且提交了一个请求或操纵性自由的悖论》[The«You are free of ...»:诱导技术顺应了歌of freedom and in a request黄金a manipulating The paradox of freedom)。国际社会心理学评论,15(1),51 - 80。
{"title":"Correction: La technique du « vous êtes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberté et soumission à une requête ou le paradoxe d’une liberté manipulatrice","authors":"A. Pascual","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.303","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.303","url":null,"abstract":"This article details a correction to the article: Pascual, A., & Gueguen, N. (2002). La technique du « vous etes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberte et soumission a une requete ou le paradoxe d’une liberte manipulatrice [The « You are free of… » technique: induction of a feeling of freedom and compliance in a request or the paradox of a manipulating freedom]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 15(1), 51–80. Resume Cet article est une correction de l’article : Pascual, A., & Gueguen, N. (2002). La technique du « vous etes libre de… » : induction d’un sentiment de liberte et soumission a une requete ou le paradoxe d’une liberte manipulatrice [The « You are free of… » technique: induction of a feeling of freedom and compliance in a request or the paradox of a manipulating freedom]. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale/International Review of Social Psychology, 15(1), 51–80.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45012545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Thirty Years of Publications in the International Review of Social Psychology (1988–2018): Genesis, Evolution and Development 《国际社会心理学评论》发表三十年(1988-2018):起源、进化与发展
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-05-17 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.203
B. Degraeve, S. Rambaud
When they became the first chief editors of the International Review of Social Psychology (IRSP), the goal of Robert Pages and Janine Larrue was to provide better access to texts and research in social psychology. Their legacy continues and extends today. For the 30th anniversary of the journal, the following contribution intends to retrace its origins and evolutions. Following an overview of the history of IRSP, we propose to analyze the characteristics of the articles that have been published during the 30 years of existence of the journal and their evolution over time. For this purpose, we developed a database—available at:  osf.io/69x47 —containing the characteristics of the articles published between 1988 and May 1, 2018 (i.e., for each year: title of the articles, volume, type, authors’ names, sex and institutional affiliation, length and associated keywords). This database can be useful to browse IRSP’s literature and facilitate reference search, but can also be used for further research on the evolution of the scientific literature in social psychology (e.g., analysis of the characteristics of the authors/published contents of the field).
当他们成为《国际社会心理学评论》(IRSP)的第一任主编时,Robert Pages和Janine Larrue的目标是提供更好的文本访问和社会心理学研究。他们的遗产延续至今。为了纪念该杂志出版30周年,以下文章旨在追溯其起源和演变。在概述《IRSP》的历史之后,我们建议分析该杂志成立30年来发表的文章的特征及其随时间的演变。为此,我们开发了一个数据库,可访问:osf.io/69x47,其中包含1988年至2018年5月1日期间发表的文章的特征(即,每年:文章标题、卷、类型、作者姓名、性别和机构隶属关系、长度和相关关键词)。该数据库可用于浏览IRSP的文献和方便参考文献搜索,但也可用于进一步研究社会心理学中科学文献的演变(例如,分析作者的特征/该领域已发表的内容)。
{"title":"Thirty Years of Publications in the International Review of Social Psychology (1988–2018): Genesis, Evolution and Development","authors":"B. Degraeve, S. Rambaud","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.203","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.203","url":null,"abstract":"When they became the first chief editors of the International Review of Social Psychology (IRSP), the goal of Robert Pages and Janine Larrue was to provide better access to texts and research in social psychology. Their legacy continues and extends today. For the 30th anniversary of the journal, the following contribution intends to retrace its origins and evolutions. Following an overview of the history of IRSP, we propose to analyze the characteristics of the articles that have been published during the 30 years of existence of the journal and their evolution over time. For this purpose, we developed a database—available at:  osf.io/69x47 —containing the characteristics of the articles published between 1988 and May 1, 2018 (i.e., for each year: title of the articles, volume, type, authors’ names, sex and institutional affiliation, length and associated keywords). This database can be useful to browse IRSP’s literature and facilitate reference search, but can also be used for further research on the evolution of the scientific literature in social psychology (e.g., analysis of the characteristics of the authors/published contents of the field).","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48533445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tweeted Anger Predicts County-Level Results of the 2016 United States Presidential Election 推特愤怒预测2016年美国总统大选的县级结果
IF 2.5 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2019-05-03 DOI: 10.5334/IRSP.256
K. Bernecker, Michael Wenzler, K. Sassenberg
In the aftermath of the 2016 United States presidential election, experts and journalists speculated that angry voters had supported the unexpected winner Donald Trump. The present study used a sample of 148 million tweets posted by U.S. citizens from across 1,347 counties, classified with regard to emotional content, to predict the election results at county level. As expected, Donald Trump received more support in counties where people tweeted more anger and negative emotions, even when various county characteristics and conservative vote choice in the preceding presidential election were controlled. These findings might be an outcome of emotional resonance—voters being attracted by political appeals that match their emotions—because Trump used more anger and negative emotion words in his campaign than the other presidential candidates in 2012 and 2016. The findings suggest that negative emotions played a critical role in the 2016 presidential election.
2016年美国总统大选结束后,专家和记者猜测,愤怒的选民支持了出乎意料的赢家唐纳德·特朗普。目前的研究使用了来自1347个县的美国公民发布的1.48亿条推文样本,根据情感内容进行分类,以预测县一级的选举结果。正如预期的那样,特朗普在推特上愤怒和负面情绪更多的县获得了更多的支持,即使控制了之前总统选举中的各种县特征和保守投票选择。这些发现可能是情绪共鸣的结果——选民会被符合他们情绪的政治诉求所吸引——因为在2012年和2016年的竞选中,特朗普比其他总统候选人使用了更多的愤怒和负面情绪词汇。研究结果表明,负面情绪在2016年总统大选中发挥了关键作用。
{"title":"Tweeted Anger Predicts County-Level Results of the 2016 United States Presidential Election","authors":"K. Bernecker, Michael Wenzler, K. Sassenberg","doi":"10.5334/IRSP.256","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/IRSP.256","url":null,"abstract":"In the aftermath of the 2016 United States presidential election, experts and journalists speculated that angry voters had supported the unexpected winner Donald Trump. The present study used a sample of 148 million tweets posted by U.S. citizens from across 1,347 counties, classified with regard to emotional content, to predict the election results at county level. As expected, Donald Trump received more support in counties where people tweeted more anger and negative emotions, even when various county characteristics and conservative vote choice in the preceding presidential election were controlled. These findings might be an outcome of emotional resonance—voters being attracted by political appeals that match their emotions—because Trump used more anger and negative emotion words in his campaign than the other presidential candidates in 2012 and 2016. The findings suggest that negative emotions played a critical role in the 2016 presidential election.","PeriodicalId":45461,"journal":{"name":"International Review of Social Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2019-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42059923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
International Review of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1