首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Research in Personality最新文献

英文 中文
Agentic collective narcissism and communal collective narcissism: Do they predict COVID-19 pandemic-related beliefs and behaviors? 代理人集体自恋和社区集体自恋:它们能预测与 COVID-19 大流行相关的信念和行为吗?
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104542
In a multinational study (61 countries; N = 15,039), we examined how collective narcissists, both agentic (ACN) and communal (CCN), reacted cognitively (through endorsement of unfounded conspiracy and health beliefs) and behaviorally (via prevention, hoarding, and prosociality) to the pandemic. Higher ACN and CCN predicted greater endorsement of COVID-19 unfounded beliefs and higher likelihood of having recently engaged in pandemic-related prevention, hoarding, and prosociality. The predictive effects of ACN and CCN were independent, suggesting construct separability. Fear positively predicted endorsement of unfounded beliefs and behaviors, but the slope of that relation was flattened when ACN and CCN were particularly high. Finally, the relation between ACN or CCN and outcomes changed across countries varying in collective fear.
在一项多国研究(61 个国家;N = 15,039)中,我们考察了集体自恋者(包括代理自恋者(ACN)和社区自恋者(CCN))是如何在认知上(通过认可毫无根据的阴谋和健康信念)和行为上(通过预防、囤积和亲社会行为)对大流行病做出反应的。较高的 ACN 和 CCN 预测了对 COVID-19 毫无根据的信念的认可程度,以及最近参与与大流行病相关的预防、囤积和亲社会行为的可能性。ACN 和 CCN 的预测效应是独立的,这表明它们具有可分性。恐惧对毫无根据的信念和行为的认可有积极的预测作用,但当 ACN 和 CCN 特别高时,这种关系的斜率趋于平缓。最后,在集体恐惧程度不同的国家,ACN 或 CCN 与结果之间的关系也发生了变化。
{"title":"Agentic collective narcissism and communal collective narcissism: Do they predict COVID-19 pandemic-related beliefs and behaviors?","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104542","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104542","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In a multinational study (61 countries; <em>N</em> = 15,039), we examined how collective narcissists, both agentic (ACN) and communal (CCN), reacted cognitively (through endorsement of unfounded conspiracy and health beliefs) and behaviorally (via prevention, hoarding, and prosociality) to the pandemic. Higher ACN and CCN predicted greater endorsement of COVID-19 unfounded beliefs and higher likelihood of having recently engaged in pandemic-related prevention, hoarding, and prosociality. The predictive effects of ACN and CCN were independent, suggesting construct separability. Fear positively predicted endorsement of unfounded beliefs and behaviors, but the slope of that relation was flattened when ACN and CCN were particularly high. Finally, the relation between ACN or CCN and outcomes changed across countries varying in collective fear.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142425919","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optimism and pessimism were prospectively associated with adaptation during the COVID-19 pandemic 在 COVID-19 大流行期间,乐观和悲观情绪与适应性的前瞻性关联
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104541

Using longitudinal data from the Health and Retirement Study, we examined the association between optimism/pessimism before the pandemic and adaptation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, optimism was associated with behaviors that reduce COVID-19 transmission and higher psychological well-being (βs>|.196|) through changes in social contacts (indirect effect βs> |.004|) and/or increases in physical activity (βs=|.01|). Separating optimism and pessimism, we found that only pessimism was associated with behaviors that reduce risk, but both optimism and pessimism were associated with psychological well-being. By investigating them in the context of new public health challenges, we found that while the presence of optimism and absence of pessimism may both be resources for well-being, the absence of pessimism may be particularly important for health-relevant behaviors.

利用健康与退休研究(Health and Retirement Study)的纵向数据,我们研究了大流行前的乐观/悲观情绪与 COVID-19 大流行期间的适应性之间的关联。总体而言,乐观情绪与通过改变社会接触(间接效应 βs>|.004|)和/或增加体育锻炼(βs=|.01|)来减少 COVID-19 传播和提高心理健康水平(βs>|.196|)的行为有关。将乐观主义和悲观主义分开后,我们发现只有悲观主义与降低风险的行为相关,但乐观主义和悲观主义都与心理健康相关。通过在新的公共卫生挑战背景下对它们进行调查,我们发现,虽然存在乐观情绪和不悲观可能都是幸福感的资源,但不悲观可能对健康相关行为尤为重要。
{"title":"Optimism and pessimism were prospectively associated with adaptation during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104541","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104541","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Using longitudinal data from the Health and Retirement Study, we examined the association between optimism/pessimism before the pandemic and adaptation during the COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, optimism was associated with behaviors that reduce COVID-19 transmission and higher psychological well-being (βs&gt;|.196|) through changes in social contacts (indirect effect βs&gt; |.004|) and/or increases in physical activity (βs=|.01|). Separating optimism and pessimism, we found that only pessimism was associated with behaviors that reduce risk, but <em>both</em> optimism and pessimism were associated with psychological well-being. By investigating them in the context of new public health challenges, we found that while the presence of optimism and absence of pessimism may both be resources for well-being, the absence of pessimism may be particularly important for health-relevant behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142272498","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age and gender differences in the value development of Dutch adults in 11 years of longitudinal data 11 年纵向数据中荷兰成年人价值观发展的年龄和性别差异
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104540

Value development over the life-span is rarely studied due to theory and data limitations. We use the LISS data, a Dutch longitudinal dataset, to study value change in adults aged 25–70 over 11 years from 2008 to 2019 (N=10,860), using the neo- socioanalyitcal model (NSM) as a theoretical framework. We find few cohort differences, differences between age groups and non-linear value change within individuals that continues until late adulthood. Gender differences in mean-levels are stable except in universalism and self-direction, while gender differences in rates of change are observed. We conclude that the NSM provides a fruitful framework to interpret value change as a maturation process toward becoming functioning members of society along gendered and age-graded normative stages.

由于理论和数据的限制,很少有人研究人一生中的价值发展。我们使用荷兰纵向数据集 LISS 数据,以新社会分析模型(NSM)为理论框架,研究了 2008 年至 2019 年 11 年间 25-70 岁成年人的价值变化(N=10,860)。我们发现队列差异、年龄组之间的差异和个体内部的非线性价值变化很小,这种变化一直持续到成年晚期。除普遍主义和自我导向外,平均水平上的性别差异是稳定的,而变化率上的性别差异是可以观察到的。我们的结论是,NSM 提供了一个富有成果的框架,可以将价值观的变化解释为一个成熟的过程,即按照性别和年龄分级的规范阶段,成为社会中起作用的一员。
{"title":"Age and gender differences in the value development of Dutch adults in 11 years of longitudinal data","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104540","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104540","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Value development over the life-span is rarely studied due to theory and data limitations. We use the LISS data, a Dutch longitudinal dataset, to study value change in adults aged 25–70 over 11 years from 2008 to 2019 (N=10,860), using the neo- socioanalyitcal model (NSM) as a theoretical framework. We find few cohort differences, differences between age groups and non-linear value change within individuals that continues until late adulthood. Gender differences in mean-levels are stable except in universalism and self-direction, while gender differences in rates of change are observed. We conclude that the NSM provides a fruitful framework to interpret value change as a maturation process toward becoming functioning members of society along gendered and age-graded normative stages.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000886/pdfft?md5=113f136b7540f7f00d14c739c7cb239f&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000886-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142272499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An investigation testing the perceptual advantage of Sensory Processing Sensitivity and its associations with the Big Five personality traits 感官处理敏感性的感知优势及其与五大人格特质的关联性测试研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104539
This study investigated whether sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) is associated with a perceptual advantage, rather than just heightened brain, emotional and behavioural reactivity. Participants (N = 222) were tested on detection and identification of visually degraded words at three levels of difficulty, and completed the Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS) and the Big Five Inventory. The positive subscale of the HSPS predicted both the detection and identification of visually degraded stimuli, and beyond the Big Five traits. This contradicts claims that SPS is solely a combination of Big Five traits. Importantly, the perceptual advantage for highly sensitives may balance the disadvantages of being easily overwhelmed by stimuli and indicates separate evolutionary advantages and strategies for high and low SPS humans and other mammals.
本研究调查了感觉处理敏感性(SPS)是否与知觉优势相关,而不仅仅是大脑、情绪和行为反应性的增强。参与者(N = 222)接受了三个难度级别的视觉降级词的检测和识别测试,并填写了高敏感人群量表(HSPS)和大五量表。高敏感性人格量表的正向分量表预测了视觉降级刺激的检测和识别能力,而且超出了大五特征的预测能力。这与 "SPS "仅仅是 "大五 "特质组合的说法相矛盾。重要的是,高度敏感者在知觉方面的优势可能会平衡容易被刺激物淹没的劣势,并表明高、低SPS人类和其他哺乳动物在进化过程中具有不同的优势和策略。
{"title":"An investigation testing the perceptual advantage of Sensory Processing Sensitivity and its associations with the Big Five personality traits","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104539","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104539","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study investigated whether sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) is associated with a perceptual advantage, rather than just heightened brain, emotional and behavioural reactivity. Participants (N = 222) were tested on detection and identification of visually degraded words at three levels of difficulty, and completed the Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS) and the Big Five Inventory. The positive subscale of the HSPS predicted both the detection and identification of visually degraded stimuli, and beyond the Big Five traits. This contradicts claims that SPS is solely a combination of Big Five traits. Importantly, the perceptual advantage for highly sensitives may balance the disadvantages of being easily overwhelmed by stimuli and indicates separate evolutionary advantages and strategies for high and low SPS humans and other mammals.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142359000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self/observer agreement in personality assessment by observers’ relationship types 观察者关系类型对人格评估中自我/观察者一致性的影响
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104529

We examined two forms of self/observer agreement (correlational and mean-level) in personality using a Dutch university student sample (N=5,405) with self-reports and observer (informant) reports from parents, siblings, friends, and partners/spouses. Correlational self/observer agreement was strong across all HEXACO-PI-R scales and across relationship types (r¯ ≥ 0.59, but highest for partners). Regarding mean-level self/observer agreement, alleged positive bias in self-reports was not observed. Only Openness showed higher means for self-reports than for observer reports across all relationship types (d = 0.37). Mean observer report scores varied by relationship: people perceived their children as more honest and less anxious and perceived their siblings as less agreeable than other observers did. Partner reports showed the closest mean-level agreement with self-reports.

我们使用荷兰大学生样本(样本数=5,405),通过自我报告和来自父母、兄弟姐妹、朋友和伴侣/配偶的观察者(信息提供者)报告,研究了人格中自我/观察者一致性的两种形式(相关性和平均水平)。在所有 HEXACO-PI-R 量表和各种关系类型中,自我/观察者的相关一致性都很强(r¯ ≥ 0.59,但伴侣的相关一致性最高)。在平均水平的自我/观察者一致性方面,没有观察到自我报告中的所谓正偏差。在所有关系类型中,只有开放性的自我报告平均值高于观察者报告平均值(d = 0.37)。观察者报告的平均分因关系而异:与其他观察者相比,人们认为子女更诚实、更不焦虑,认为兄弟姐妹更不合群。伴侣报告与自我报告的平均水平最接近。
{"title":"Self/observer agreement in personality assessment by observers’ relationship types","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104529","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104529","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined two forms of self/observer agreement (correlational and mean-level) in personality using a Dutch university student sample (<em>N</em>=5,405) with self-reports and observer (informant) reports from parents, siblings, friends, and partners/spouses. Correlational self/observer agreement was strong across all HEXACO-PI-R scales and across relationship types (<span><math><mrow><mover><mrow><mi>r</mi></mrow><mrow><mo>¯</mo></mrow></mover></mrow></math></span> ≥ 0.59, but highest for partners). Regarding mean-level self/observer agreement, alleged positive bias in self-reports was not observed. Only Openness showed higher means for self-reports than for observer reports across all relationship types (<em>d</em> = 0.37). Mean observer report scores varied by relationship: people perceived their children as more honest and less anxious and perceived their siblings as less agreeable than other observers did. Partner reports showed the closest mean-level agreement with self-reports.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000771/pdfft?md5=b35202e898398e84438eb6c24081322c&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000771-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142241123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looking through a glass onion: Exploring the validity of eye-tracking technology in capturing self-directed attention 透过玻璃洋葱看世界探索眼动追踪技术在捕捉自主注意力方面的有效性
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104538

Self-directed attention is a central aspect in most psychological models in the clinical, social and personality literature. However, precise measures of self-directed attention are lacking. Building on recent methodological developments, the present study (N=104) provides an exploratory assessment of the Incidental Mirror Exposure (I-ME) paradigm combining reflective screens with eye-tracking devices to measure self-directed attention. Personality traits associated with self-directed attention were assessed to evaluate the theoretical validity of basic oculometric measures. We additionally suggest a novel measure of self-focus integrating time spent looking at the self-reflecting area of the screen and depth of the gaze looking through the screen. Results underline the relevance of eye-tracking paradigms to capture maladaptive self-directed attention such as social anxiety, vulnerable narcissism, and self-absorption.

自我定向注意是临床、社会和人格文献中大多数心理学模型的核心内容。然而,目前还缺乏对自我定向注意的精确测量。本研究(N=104)以最近的方法论发展为基础,对偶发镜像暴露(I-ME)范式进行了探索性评估,该范式结合了反光屏幕和眼动追踪设备来测量自我导向注意。我们评估了与自我定向注意相关的人格特质,以评价基本眼动测量的理论有效性。此外,我们还提出了一种新的自我专注度测量方法,该方法综合了注视屏幕自我反射区域的时间和注视屏幕的深度。研究结果强调了眼动追踪范式在捕捉不良自我定向注意力(如社交焦虑、脆弱自恋和自我吸收)方面的相关性。
{"title":"Looking through a glass onion: Exploring the validity of eye-tracking technology in capturing self-directed attention","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104538","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104538","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Self-directed attention is a central aspect in most psychological models in the clinical, social and personality literature. However, precise measures of self-directed attention are lacking. Building on recent methodological developments, the present study (N=104) provides an exploratory assessment of the Incidental Mirror Exposure (I-ME) paradigm combining reflective screens with eye-tracking devices to measure self-directed attention. Personality traits associated with self-directed attention were assessed to evaluate the theoretical validity of basic oculometric measures. We additionally suggest a novel measure of self-focus integrating time spent looking at the self-reflecting area of the screen and depth of the gaze looking through the screen. Results underline the relevance of eye-tracking paradigms to capture maladaptive self-directed attention such as social anxiety, vulnerable narcissism, and self-absorption.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000862/pdfft?md5=419a7d76936935c17f90983c9240beb1&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000862-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142150143","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using multi-rater and test-retest data to detect overlap within and between psychological scales 利用多测评者和重复测试数据检测心理量表内部和之间的重叠情况
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104530

Correlations estimated in single-source data provide uninterpretable estimates of empirical overlap between scales. We describe a model to adjust correlations for errors and biases using test–retest and multi-rater data and compare adjusted correlations among individual items with their human-rated semantic similarity (SS). We expected adjusted correlations to predict SS better than unadjusted correlations and exceed SS in absolute magnitude. While unadjusted and adjusted correlations predicted SS rankings equally well across all items, adjusted correlations were superior where items were judged most semantically redundant in meaning. Retest- and agreement-adjusted correlations were usually higher than SS, whereas unadjusted correlations often underestimated SS. We discuss uses of test–retest and multi-rater data for identifying construct redundancy and argue SS often underestimates variables’ empirical overlap.

单源数据中估算的相关性对量表之间的经验重叠提供了无法解释的估算。我们描述了一个利用测试-重测和多评定者数据调整相关性以消除误差和偏差的模型,并将调整后的单个项目间相关性与人类评定的语义相似性(SS)进行比较。我们期望调整后的相关性能比未调整的相关性更好地预测语义相似性,并在绝对值上超过语义相似性。虽然未经调整的相关性和调整后的相关性对所有项目的 SS 排名的预测效果相同,但调整后的相关性在项目被判定为语义冗余度最高时更胜一筹。重测和一致性调整相关通常高于 SS,而未调整相关往往低估了 SS。我们讨论了使用重测和多评定者数据来识别建构冗余的问题,并认为 SS 往往低估了变量的经验重叠性。
{"title":"Using multi-rater and test-retest data to detect overlap within and between psychological scales","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104530","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104530","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Correlations estimated in single-source data provide uninterpretable estimates of empirical overlap between scales. We describe a model to adjust correlations for errors and biases using test–retest and multi-rater data and compare adjusted correlations among individual items with their human-rated semantic similarity (<em>SS</em>). We expected adjusted correlations to predict <em>SS</em> better than unadjusted correlations and exceed <em>SS</em> in absolute magnitude. While unadjusted and adjusted correlations predicted <em>SS</em> rankings equally well across all items, adjusted correlations were superior where items were judged most semantically redundant in meaning. Retest- and agreement-adjusted correlations were usually higher than <em>SS</em>, whereas unadjusted correlations often underestimated <em>SS</em>. We discuss uses of test–retest and multi-rater data for identifying construct redundancy and argue <em>SS</em> often underestimates variables’ empirical overlap.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142228505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to measure and model personality traits in everyday life: A qualitative analysis of 300 big five personality items 如何测量和模拟日常生活中的人格特质:对 300 个五大人格项目的定性分析
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104528

Personality traits are often described with reference to momentary patterns in experiences and behaviors. In this paper, we consider how such personality manifestations can be measured and modeled more directly within intensive-longitudinal research. Specifically, we evaluated 300 items from the international personality item pool with respect to: a) how the described behavior relates to situational factors; and b) which statistic most accurately quantifies the implied pattern. Our results give rise to three critical observations. First, most patterns only occur within certain contexts, rather than being context independent. Second, traits are mostly described as dynamic patterns on differing timescales. Third, the mean of a person across many repeated measures is poorly suited to capture most patterns of personality in daily life.

对人格特质的描述通常是参照经验和行为的瞬间模式。在本文中,我们考虑了如何在密集纵向研究中更直接地测量和模拟此类人格表现。具体来说,我们对国际人格项目库中的 300 个项目进行了评估:a) 所描述的行为与情境因素的关系;b) 哪种统计量能最准确地量化隐含的模式。我们的研究结果提出了三个重要观点。首先,大多数模式只出现在特定情境中,而非独立于情境。第二,特质大多被描述为不同时间尺度上的动态模式。第三,一个人在多次重复测量中的平均值并不适合捕捉日常生活中的大多数人格模式。
{"title":"How to measure and model personality traits in everyday life: A qualitative analysis of 300 big five personality items","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104528","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104528","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Personality traits are often described with reference to momentary patterns in experiences and behaviors. In this paper, we consider how such personality manifestations can be measured and modeled more directly within intensive-longitudinal research. Specifically, we evaluated 300 items from the international personality item pool with respect to: a) how the described behavior relates to situational factors; and b) which statistic most accurately quantifies the implied pattern. Our results give rise to three critical observations. First, most patterns only occur within certain contexts, rather than being context independent. Second, traits are mostly described as dynamic patterns on differing timescales. Third, the mean of a person across many repeated measures is poorly suited to capture most patterns of personality in daily life.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S009265662400076X/pdfft?md5=52ffd44231b693585528b13ad074e194&pid=1-s2.0-S009265662400076X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142076158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between five factor model and impulsive personality traits and PTSD symptoms: Findings from the AURORA study 五因素模型和冲动型人格特质与创伤后应激障碍症状之间的纵向联系:AURORA 研究的结果
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104524

We used data from the Advancing Understanding of Recovery after Trauma (AURORA) study to investigate prospective links between five factor model and impulsive personality traits and PTSD symptoms at baseline (N = 2943), three-months post-trauma (N = 2400), and one-year post-trauma (N = 1591) in individuals recruited from emergency departments within 72 h of trauma exposure. Neuroticism and Negative Urgency bore the largest relations (rs > 0.30) to nearly all individual PTSD symptoms and symptom total at all time points. Neuroticism was an incremental predictor of every PTSD symptom at each time point. Low Agreeableness and low Conscientiousness were incremental predictors of several PTSD symptoms. These findings highlight personality assessment as an efficient, effective screening tool for PTSD risk.

我们利用 "促进对创伤后康复的理解"(AURORA)研究的数据,调查了从急诊科招募的、在创伤暴露后 72 小时内出现的五因素模型和冲动型人格特质与创伤后应激障碍症状在基线(2943 人)、创伤后三个月(2400 人)和创伤后一年(1591 人)的前瞻性联系。在所有时间点上,神经质和消极急迫性与几乎所有创伤后应激障碍症状和症状总数的关系最大(rs > 0.30)。神经质是每个时间点上每种创伤后应激障碍症状的递增预测因子。低宜人性和低自觉性是几种创伤后应激障碍症状的递增预测因子。这些发现凸显了人格评估是一种高效、有效的创伤后应激障碍风险筛查工具。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between five factor model and impulsive personality traits and PTSD symptoms: Findings from the AURORA study","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104524","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104524","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We used data from the Advancing Understanding of Recovery after Trauma (AURORA) study to investigate prospective links between five factor model and impulsive personality traits and PTSD symptoms at baseline (<em>N</em> = 2943),<!--> <!-->three-months post-trauma (<em>N</em> = 2400), and one-year post-trauma (<em>N</em> = 1591) in individuals<!--> <!-->recruited from emergency departments within 72 h of trauma exposure. Neuroticism and Negative Urgency bore the largest relations (<em>r</em>s &gt; 0.30) to nearly all individual PTSD symptoms and symptom total at all time points. Neuroticism was an incremental predictor of every PTSD symptom at each time point. Low Agreeableness and low Conscientiousness were incremental predictors<!--> <!-->of several PTSD symptoms. These findings highlight personality assessment as an efficient, effective screening tool for PTSD risk.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142011514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating everyday sadistic consumption within dark tetrad 调查黑暗四重奏中的日常虐待性消费
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104527

An experimental study on a non-WEIRD sample (N = 337; Turkish consumers) was conducted on how the traces of sadism can be empirically found in daily consumption behaviors. Intention to visit a tourist destination in which a cruel story presented for the experimental group (vs. control group) analyzed. It is found that sadism initially negatively predicts the intention to visit when controlling for other members of the dark tetrad; however, when a cruel story related to the destination is presented, this negative effect disappears. Further, when participants were asked to create a memoir about the destination, sadism predicted the intention to add the cruel story about the destination and share the memoir with others when controlling other members of the dark tetrad.

我们对非 WEIRD 样本(N = 337;土耳其消费者)进行了一项实验研究,探讨如何从日常消费行为中发现虐待狂的痕迹。研究分析了实验组(与对照组)访问旅游景点的意向,在该旅游景点中,实验组展示了一个残酷的故事。结果发现,当控制了黑暗四分体的其他成员时,虐待狂最初会对旅游意向产生负面预测;然而,当出现与旅游目的地相关的残酷故事时,这种负面影响就会消失。此外,当要求受试者创作关于目的地的回忆录时,在控制其他黑暗四分体成员的情况下,虐待狂会预测受试者添加关于目的地的残酷故事并与他人分享回忆录的意愿。
{"title":"Investigating everyday sadistic consumption within dark tetrad","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104527","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104527","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>An experimental study on a non-WEIRD sample (N = 337; Turkish consumers) was conducted on how the traces of sadism can be empirically found in daily consumption behaviors. Intention to visit a tourist destination in which a cruel story presented for the experimental group (vs. control group) analyzed. It is found that sadism initially negatively predicts the intention to visit when controlling for other members of the dark tetrad; however, when a cruel story related to the destination is presented, this negative effect disappears. Further, when participants were asked to create a memoir about the destination, sadism predicted the intention to add the cruel story about the destination and share the memoir with others when controlling other members of the dark tetrad.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142011516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Research in Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1