首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Research in Personality最新文献

英文 中文
Intolerance of uncertainty predicts indecisiveness and safety behavior in real-life decision making: Results from an experience sampling study 对不确定性的不容忍度可预测现实决策中的优柔寡断和安全行为:经验取样研究的结果
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490
Helmut Appel , Julia Krasko , Maike Luhmann , Alexander L. Gerlach

In decision making, intolerance of uncertainty (IU) may be linked to safety behavior, which could ironically maintain IU and, thus, foster indecisiveness. In a smartphone-based experience sampling study, 247 participants described their real-life decisions six times per day and rated (a) their situational indecisiveness, (b) decision characteristics, (c) situational IU, and (d) problematic safety behaviors. Participants higher in dispositional IU reported more indecisiveness and engaged in more problematic safety behavior across measurements. The same relationships were observed with situational IU at the level of individual measurements. Engaging in more problematic safety behaviors during the first days predicted indecisiveness on the last day, mediated by IU. The results demonstrate the real-life relevance of the relationship between IU, indecisiveness, and safety behavior.

在决策过程中,对不确定性的不容忍度(IU)可能与安全行为有关,而安全行为可能会具有讽刺意味地维持不确定性,从而助长优柔寡断。在一项基于智能手机的经验取样研究中,247 名参与者每天六次描述他们在现实生活中做出的决定,并对以下方面进行评分:(a)他们的情境优柔寡断性;(b)决策特征;(c)情境 IU;以及(d)有问题的安全行为。倾向性 IU 值较高的受试者在各次测量中表现出更多的优柔寡断,并参与了更多有问题的安全行为。在个人测量水平上,也观察到了与情境性 IU 的相同关系。在前几天参与更多有问题的安全行为可以预测最后一天的优柔寡断,而这是由 IU 调解的。这些结果表明,IU、优柔寡断和安全行为之间的关系与现实生活息息相关。
{"title":"Intolerance of uncertainty predicts indecisiveness and safety behavior in real-life decision making: Results from an experience sampling study","authors":"Helmut Appel ,&nbsp;Julia Krasko ,&nbsp;Maike Luhmann ,&nbsp;Alexander L. Gerlach","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In decision making, intolerance of uncertainty (IU) may be linked to safety behavior, which could ironically maintain IU and, thus, foster indecisiveness. In a smartphone-based experience sampling study, 247 participants described their real-life decisions six times per day and rated (a) their situational indecisiveness, (b) decision characteristics, (c) situational IU, and (d) problematic safety behaviors. Participants higher in dispositional IU reported more indecisiveness and engaged in more problematic safety behavior across measurements. The same relationships were observed with situational IU at the level of individual measurements. Engaging in more problematic safety behaviors during the first days predicted indecisiveness on the last day, mediated by IU. The results demonstrate the real-life relevance of the relationship between IU, indecisiveness, and safety behavior.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000382/pdfft?md5=9f092953545ce88d8ebe5c10e351b44f&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000382-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140399112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does narrating the life story predict changes in personality traits and characteristics? 讲述人生故事能否预测人格特质和特征的变化?
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489
Rebekka Weidmann , Janina Larissa Bühler , Jenna Wünsche , Alexander Grob

To measure narratives, individuals often participate in a life story interview, which bears similarity to a narrative therapeutic approach. Given that clinical interventions were shown to impact change in personality traits and characteristics, the present study explored whether narrating one’s life story is also linked to such changes. The pre-registered study compared 123 life-story-interview participants (Mage = 35.44 years) with 123 control-sample participants (Mage = 35.50 years). We assessed participants’ personality traits, optimism, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and affect before and 1 and 3 years after the interview. The findings suggest that the life story interview does not lead to consistent changes in personality traits and characteristics, indicating that this interview setting developed for measurement is different from an intervention.

为了测量叙事,个人通常会参加人生故事访谈,这与叙事治疗方法有相似之处。鉴于临床干预对人格特质和特征的改变有影响,本研究探讨了叙述自己的人生故事是否也与这种改变有关。这项预先登记的研究比较了 123 名生活故事访谈参与者(年龄为 35.44 岁)和 123 名对照样本参与者(年龄为 35.50 岁)。我们在访谈前、访谈后 1 年和 3 年评估了参与者的人格特质、乐观程度、自尊、生活满意度和情感。研究结果表明,生命故事访谈不会导致人格特质和特征发生一致的变化,这表明这种为测量而开发的访谈设置不同于干预措施。
{"title":"Does narrating the life story predict changes in personality traits and characteristics?","authors":"Rebekka Weidmann ,&nbsp;Janina Larissa Bühler ,&nbsp;Jenna Wünsche ,&nbsp;Alexander Grob","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To measure narratives, individuals often participate in a life story interview, which bears similarity to a narrative therapeutic approach. Given that clinical interventions were shown to impact change in personality traits and characteristics, the present study explored whether narrating one’s life story is also linked to such changes. The pre-registered study compared 123 life-story-interview participants (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> <em>=</em> 35.44 years) with 123 control-sample participants (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> <em>=</em> 35.50 years). We assessed participants’ personality traits, optimism, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and affect before and 1 and 3 years after the interview. The findings suggest that the life story interview does not lead to consistent changes in personality traits and characteristics, indicating that this interview setting developed for measurement is different from an intervention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140332976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Further exploring the impact of cumulative lifetime adversity on life satisfaction, psychological flourishing, and depressive symptoms 进一步探讨一生中累积的逆境对生活满意度、心理健康和抑郁症状的影响
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488
Stephanie A. Bossert , Eranda Jayawickreme , Laura E.R. Blackie , Veronica T. Cole

Current research provides conflicting views of the relationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and dispositional functioning. We examined this relationship in a sample of adults (N = 1009), conducting the analysis three ways: by utilizing a sum score of adversity across all ages; examining events that occurred before and after age 18 separately; and assessing this relationship using empirically-derived domains of adversity. Results indicate that the type of adversity has a unique impact on future well-being. These findings underscore that adversity’s unique impact on well-being is influenced by the way in which adversity is operationalized, more so than by the timing of adverse events. Results highlight the importance of clarifying the nuanced relationship between adverse life events and mental health in future research.

目前的研究对一生中累积的逆境与性格功能之间的关系提供了相互矛盾的观点。我们对成人样本(N = 1009)中的这一关系进行了研究,分析方法有三种:利用所有年龄段的逆境总分;分别研究 18 岁之前和之后发生的事件;利用从经验中得出的逆境领域评估这一关系。结果表明,逆境的类型对未来的幸福感有独特的影响。这些发现强调,逆境对幸福感的独特影响受到逆境的操作方式的影响,而不是逆境事件发生的时间。研究结果凸显了在未来研究中阐明逆境与心理健康之间微妙关系的重要性。
{"title":"Further exploring the impact of cumulative lifetime adversity on life satisfaction, psychological flourishing, and depressive symptoms","authors":"Stephanie A. Bossert ,&nbsp;Eranda Jayawickreme ,&nbsp;Laura E.R. Blackie ,&nbsp;Veronica T. Cole","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Current research provides conflicting views of the relationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and dispositional functioning. We examined this relationship in a sample of adults (<em>N</em> = 1009), conducting the analysis three ways: by utilizing a sum score of adversity across all ages; examining events that occurred before and after age 18 separately; and assessing this relationship using empirically-derived domains of adversity. Results indicate that the type of adversity has a unique impact on future well-being. These findings underscore that adversity’s unique impact on well-being is influenced by the way in which adversity is operationalized, more so than by the timing of adverse events. Results highlight the importance of clarifying the nuanced relationship between adverse life events and mental health in future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140282139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterizing stress processes by linking big five personality states, traits, and day-to-day stressors 将五大人格状态、特质和日常压力源联系起来,描述压力过程
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487
Whitney R. Ringwald , Sienna R. Nielsen , Janan Mostajabi , Colin E. Vize , Tessa van den Berg , Stephen B. Manuck , Anna L. Marsland , Aidan G.C. Wright

The accumulation of day-to-day stressors can impact mental and physical health. How people respond to stressful events is a key mechanism responsible for the effects of stress, and individual differences in stress responses can either perpetuate or prevent negative consequences. Most research on daily stress processes has focused on affective responses to stressors, but stress responses can involve more than just affect (e.g., behavior, cognitions). Additionally, most research has studied the role of neuroticism in shaping those responses, but many other individual differences are associated with stress. In this study, we more broadly characterized daily stress processes by expanding the nomological networks of stress responses to include Big Five personality states. We also linked those stress responses to all Big Five traits, as well as individual differences in stress variety, severity, and controllability. We studied a sample of participants (N = 1,090) who reported on stressful events, their appraisal of events in terms of severity and controllability, and their Big Five personality states daily for 8–10 days (N = 8,870 observations). Multi-level structural equation models were used to separate how characteristics of the perceived stressful situation and characteristics of the person play into daily stress processes. Results showed that (1) all Big Five personality states shift in response to perceived stress, (2) all Big Five personality traits relate to average levels of perceived stress variety, severity, and controllability, (3) individual differences in personality and average perceived stress variety and perceived severity relate to the strength of personality state responses to daily stress, albeit in a more limited fashion. Our results point to new pathways by which stressors affect people in everyday life and begin to clarify processes that may explain individual differences in risk or resilience to the harmful effects of stress.

日常压力的积累会影响身心健康。人们如何应对压力事件是造成压力影响的一个关键机制,而压力反应的个体差异可以延续或防止负面影响。大多数有关日常压力过程的研究都侧重于对压力源的情感反应,但压力反应可能不仅仅涉及情感(如行为、认知)。此外,大多数研究都研究了神经质在形成这些反应中的作用,但许多其他个体差异也与压力有关。在本研究中,我们通过扩展压力反应的名义网络,将大五人格状态纳入其中,从而更广泛地描述了日常压力过程。我们还将这些压力反应与所有五大性格特征以及压力种类、严重程度和可控性方面的个体差异联系起来。我们对参与者样本(N = 1,090)进行了研究,这些参与者在 8-10 天内每天报告压力事件、他们对事件严重性和可控性的评价以及他们的大五人格状态(N = 8,870 个观察值)。我们使用多层次结构方程模型来区分感知到的压力情境特征和个人特征在日常压力过程中的作用。结果表明:(1) 所有的大五人格状态都会对感知到的压力做出反应,(2) 所有的大五人格特质都与感知到的压力种类、严重程度和可控性的平均水平有关,(3) 个体的人格差异以及平均感知到的压力种类和严重程度与人格状态对日常压力反应的强度有关,尽管这种关系比较有限。我们的研究结果指出了压力因素在日常生活中对人们产生影响的新途径,并开始阐明可能解释个体差异对压力有害影响的风险或复原力的过程。
{"title":"Characterizing stress processes by linking big five personality states, traits, and day-to-day stressors","authors":"Whitney R. Ringwald ,&nbsp;Sienna R. Nielsen ,&nbsp;Janan Mostajabi ,&nbsp;Colin E. Vize ,&nbsp;Tessa van den Berg ,&nbsp;Stephen B. Manuck ,&nbsp;Anna L. Marsland ,&nbsp;Aidan G.C. Wright","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The accumulation of day-to-day stressors can impact mental and physical health. How people respond to stressful events is a key mechanism responsible for the effects of stress, and individual differences in stress responses can either perpetuate or prevent negative consequences. Most research on daily stress processes has focused on affective responses to stressors, but stress responses can involve more than just affect (e.g., behavior, cognitions). Additionally, most research has studied the role of neuroticism in shaping those responses, but many other individual differences are associated with stress. In this study, we more broadly characterized daily stress processes by expanding the nomological networks of stress responses to include Big Five personality states. We also linked those stress responses to all Big Five traits, as well as individual differences in stress variety, severity, and controllability. We studied a sample of participants (<em>N</em> = 1,090) who reported on stressful events, their appraisal of events in terms of severity and controllability, and their Big Five personality states daily for 8–10 days (<em>N</em> = 8,870 observations). Multi-level structural equation models were used to separate how characteristics of the perceived stressful situation and characteristics of the person play into daily stress processes. Results showed that (1) all Big Five personality states shift in response to perceived stress, (2) all Big Five personality traits relate to average levels of perceived stress variety, severity, and controllability, (3) individual differences in personality and average perceived stress variety and perceived severity relate to the strength of personality state responses to daily stress, albeit in a more limited fashion. Our results point to new pathways by which stressors affect people in everyday life and begin to clarify processes that may explain individual differences in risk or resilience to the harmful effects of stress.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140162695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A multi-method study of interpersonal complementarity and mentalization 对人际互补性和心智化的多方法研究
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104478
A. Esin Asan , Aaron L. Pincus , Emily B. Ansell

Research finds cross-sectional relationships between mentalizing impairments and maladaptive personality traits. The current study connects mentalizing impairments to dynamic interpersonal processes using a multi-method design. A sample of 218 participants completed the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC; Dziobek et al., 2006) to assess mentalizing ability. Subsequently, participants rated their agentic and communal behavior and their perception of interaction partners’ agentic and communal behavior over 21-days. Mentalizing ability moderated the within-person relationship between behavior and perception for both agency and communion. Worse performance on the MASC was associated with weaker interpersonal complementarity, suggesting that mentalizing impairments lead to deviations from expected patterns of behavior and perception across interpersonal situations. These findings confirm the assumption of Contemporary Integrative Interpersonal Theory that mentalization impairments can disrupt normative interpersonal processes (Cain et al., 2024, Pincus and Hopwood, 2012).

研究发现,心理化障碍与适应不良人格特质之间存在交叉关系。本研究采用多种方法设计,将心理化障碍与动态人际交往过程联系起来。218名参与者填写了社会认知评估问卷(MASC),以评估心智化能力。随后,参与者在 21 天内对自己的代理行为和交往行为以及他们对交往伙伴的代理行为和交往行为的感知进行了评分。心智化能力调节了代理和交往行为与感知之间的人际关系。MASC成绩较差与人际互补性较弱有关,这表明心智化障碍会导致人际交往中的行为和感知偏离预期模式。这些研究结果证实了当代整合人际理论的假设,即心智障碍会破坏规范的人际交往过程()。
{"title":"A multi-method study of interpersonal complementarity and mentalization","authors":"A. Esin Asan ,&nbsp;Aaron L. Pincus ,&nbsp;Emily B. Ansell","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104478","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104478","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research finds cross-sectional relationships between mentalizing impairments and maladaptive personality traits. The current study connects mentalizing impairments to dynamic interpersonal processes using a multi-method design. A sample of 218 participants completed the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC; <span>Dziobek et al., 2006</span>) to assess mentalizing ability. Subsequently, participants rated their agentic and communal behavior and their perception of interaction partners’ agentic and communal behavior over 21-days. Mentalizing ability moderated the within-person relationship between behavior and perception for both agency and communion. Worse performance on the MASC was associated with weaker interpersonal complementarity, suggesting that mentalizing impairments lead to deviations from expected patterns of behavior and perception across interpersonal situations. These findings confirm the assumption of Contemporary Integrative Interpersonal Theory that mentalization impairments can disrupt normative interpersonal processes (<span>Cain et al., 2024</span>, <span>Pincus and Hopwood, 2012</span>).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140129096","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Paradise for the self-interested? The association between economic inequality and the Dark Triad 自利者的天堂?经济不平等与黑暗三合会之间的关联
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104477
Xueli Zhu , Lei Cheng , Shijiang Zuo , Ke Yao , Fang Wang

Does an environment of high economic inequality encourage and breed the Dark Triad traits (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy)? In four studies, using correlational and experimental methods, we sought to elucidate the nexus between economic inequality perception and the Dark Triad. Study 1 (N = 454) indicated that individuals who perceive greater inequality score higher on the Dark Triad traits (except for psychopathy). When living in an economically unequal context, people tend to endorse the behaviors and mindsets of the Dark Triad traits except for psychopathy (Study 2; N = 199). People considered residents in an economically unequal society as high on the Dark Triad traits (Study 3; N = 159). Moreover, the Dark Triad traits (except for psychopathy) are considered as adaptive, allowing people to obtain more benefits in an economically unequal society (Study 4; N = 157). This study extends the determinants of the Dark Triad traits to the economic environment, thus enriching our understanding of the Dark Triad and its distal shaping factor.

高度经济不平等的环境是否会鼓励和滋生黑暗三合会特质(即马基雅维利主义、自恋和心理变态)?在四项研究中,我们采用了相关和实验方法,试图阐明经济不平等感与黑暗三合会之间的关系。研究 1(N = 454)表明,认为不平等程度越高的人在黑暗三联征上得分越高(心理变态除外)。当生活在经济不平等的环境中时,除了心理变态外,人们倾向于赞同黑暗三联征的行为和心态(研究 2;N = 199)。人们认为经济不平等社会中的居民具有较高的黑暗三合会特质(研究 3;N = 159)。此外,人们认为黑暗三合会特质(心理变态除外)具有适应性,可以让人们在经济不平等的社会中获得更多利益(研究 4;N = 157)。这项研究将黑暗三合会特质的决定因素扩展到了经济环境,从而丰富了我们对黑暗三合会及其远端塑造因素的理解。
{"title":"Paradise for the self-interested? The association between economic inequality and the Dark Triad","authors":"Xueli Zhu ,&nbsp;Lei Cheng ,&nbsp;Shijiang Zuo ,&nbsp;Ke Yao ,&nbsp;Fang Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104477","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104477","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Does an environment of high economic inequality encourage and breed the Dark Triad traits (i.e., Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy)? In four studies, using correlational and experimental methods, we sought to elucidate the nexus between economic inequality perception and the Dark Triad. Study 1 (<em>N</em> = 454) indicated that individuals who perceive greater inequality score higher on the Dark Triad traits (except for psychopathy). When living in an economically unequal context, people tend to endorse the behaviors and mindsets of the Dark Triad traits except for psychopathy (Study 2; <em>N</em> = 199). People considered residents in an economically unequal society as high on the Dark Triad traits (Study 3; <em>N</em> = 159). Moreover, the Dark Triad traits (except for psychopathy) are considered as adaptive, allowing people to obtain more benefits in an economically unequal society (Study 4; <em>N</em> = 157). This study extends the determinants of the Dark Triad traits to the economic environment, thus enriching our understanding of the Dark Triad and its distal shaping factor.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140096129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neuroticism, emotional stress reactivity and recovery in daily life: Examining extraversion and openness as moderators 神经质、情绪压力反应性和日常生活中的恢复:研究作为调节因素的外向性和开放性
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104474
Anna J. Lücke , Oliver K. Schilling , Ute Kunzmann , Denis Gerstorf , Martin Katzorreck-Gierden , Christiane A. Hoppmann , Gloria Luong , Gert G. Wagner , Michaela Riediger , Cornelia Wrzus

Emotional stressor reactivity and recovery from stressors are associated with the personality trait neuroticism. We examined whether higher extraversion or openness might buffer these associations in daily life. Participants from two age-heterogeneous samples (lifespan: n = 364, aged 14–88 years; late adulthood: n = 170, aged 66–89 years) answered personality questionnaires and reported their momentary negative affect (NA) and stressors six times per day over nine or seven days, respectively. Higher neuroticism was associated with higher overall NA in both samples, but with more pronounced stressor reactivity only in the late adulthood sample. Neither extraversion nor openness moderated associations between neuroticism and stressor reactivity or recovery. We discuss the role of different personality traits in stress processes for different age groups.

情绪压力反应性和从压力中恢复与人格特质神经质有关。我们研究了较高的外向性或开放性是否会在日常生活中缓冲这些关联。来自两个年龄异质样本(生命期:n = 364,14-88 岁;成年后期:n = 170,66-89 岁)的参与者回答了人格问卷,并分别在九天或七天内每天六次报告他们的瞬间负面情绪(NA)和压力。在两个样本中,较高的神经质都与较高的总体负性情绪有关,但只有在晚年样本中才与更明显的压力反应有关。外向性和开放性都不能调节神经质与压力反应性或恢复之间的关系。我们讨论了不同年龄组的不同人格特质在压力过程中的作用。
{"title":"Neuroticism, emotional stress reactivity and recovery in daily life: Examining extraversion and openness as moderators","authors":"Anna J. Lücke ,&nbsp;Oliver K. Schilling ,&nbsp;Ute Kunzmann ,&nbsp;Denis Gerstorf ,&nbsp;Martin Katzorreck-Gierden ,&nbsp;Christiane A. Hoppmann ,&nbsp;Gloria Luong ,&nbsp;Gert G. Wagner ,&nbsp;Michaela Riediger ,&nbsp;Cornelia Wrzus","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104474","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104474","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Emotional stressor reactivity and recovery from stressors are associated with the personality trait neuroticism. We examined whether higher extraversion or openness might buffer these associations in daily life. Participants from two age-heterogeneous samples (lifespan: <em>n</em> = 364, aged 14–88 years; late adulthood: <em>n</em> = 170, aged 66–89 years) answered personality questionnaires and reported their momentary negative affect (NA) and stressors six times per day over nine or seven days, respectively. Higher neuroticism was associated with higher overall NA in both samples, but with more pronounced stressor reactivity only in the late adulthood sample. Neither extraversion nor openness moderated associations between neuroticism and stressor reactivity or recovery. We discuss the role of different personality traits in stress processes for different age groups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000229/pdfft?md5=e8a89a7ccd15f9bb3fec31fe4f68bda6&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000229-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140042618","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cognitive complexity of a happy life, a meaningful life, and a psychologically rich life 幸福生活、有意义的生活和丰富心理生活的认知复杂性
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104475
Shigehiro Oishi , Erin Westgate , Youngjae Cha

Is a psychologically rich life a more cognitively complex one? In two studies, we explored whether individual differences in happiness, meaning in life, and psychological richness would be differentially associated with information processing styles such as attributional complexity, holism, and essentialism. In Study 1 (N = 436), we found that psychological richness was associated with greater attributional complexity, holism, and less essentialism, whereas happiness was associated with less attributional complexity. Meaning was also associated with attributional complexity but unrelated to holism and essentialism. In Study 2 (N = 516), a pre-registered replication, we again found that psychological richness was associated with more attributional complexity and holism, and marginally less essentialism. In contrast, happiness and meaning were unrelated to attributional complexity, holism, and essentialism. Across two studies, then, psychological richness was consistently associated with more attributional complexity and holism, whereas happiness and meaning were not. The key findings remained largely the same after controlling for all the Big Five personality traits and demographic variables.

心理丰富的人生是否在认知上更加复杂?在两项研究中,我们探讨了幸福感、人生意义和心理丰富度的个体差异是否与归因复杂性、整体主义和本质主义等信息处理风格有不同的关联。在研究 1(N = 436)中,我们发现心理丰富度与较高的归因复杂性、整体主义和较低的本质主义相关,而幸福感则与较低的归因复杂性相关。意义也与归因复杂性有关,但与整体主义和本质主义无关。在研究 2(N = 516)中,我们再次发现,心理丰富度与更多的归因复杂性和整体主义相关,而与较少的本质主义相关。相反,幸福感和意义与归因复杂性、整体主义和本质主义无关。因此,在两项研究中,心理丰富性始终与更多的归因复杂性和整体主义相关,而幸福感和意义则无关。在控制了所有五大人格特质和人口统计学变量后,主要研究结果基本保持不变。
{"title":"The cognitive complexity of a happy life, a meaningful life, and a psychologically rich life","authors":"Shigehiro Oishi ,&nbsp;Erin Westgate ,&nbsp;Youngjae Cha","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104475","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104475","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Is a psychologically rich life a more cognitively complex one? In two studies, we explored whether individual differences in happiness, meaning in life, and psychological richness would be differentially associated with information processing styles such as attributional complexity, holism, and essentialism. In Study 1 (<em>N</em> = 436), we found that psychological richness was associated with greater attributional complexity, holism, and less essentialism, whereas happiness was associated with less attributional complexity. Meaning was also associated with attributional complexity but unrelated to holism and essentialism. In Study 2 (<em>N</em> = 516), a pre-registered replication, we again found that psychological richness was associated with more attributional complexity and holism, and marginally less essentialism. In contrast, happiness and meaning were unrelated to attributional complexity, holism, and essentialism. Across two studies, then, psychological richness was consistently associated with more attributional complexity and holism, whereas happiness and meaning were not. The key findings remained largely the same after controlling for all the Big Five personality traits and demographic variables.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140096128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who helps whom in times of crisis? An investigation of actual donations to two groups of earthquake victims 危机时刻谁帮助谁?对两组地震灾民实际捐款情况的调查
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104476
Büsra Elif Yelbuz, Isabel Thielmann

This pre-registered study assessed actual donations to two groups of victims of the earthquake in Turkey and Syria in February 2023 while considering various theoretically-relevant contextual and personality factors as determinants of donations. In a diverse German online sample (N = 496), most participants (62.9 %) donated something, and only few (24.0 %) were selective in their donations, donating more to one group of victims than to the other. Dispositional honesty-humility added to the prediction of donation behavior beyond contextual factors. Selective donations, however, were largely driven by (contextual) perceptions of need. Overall, our findings provide novel insights into donation behavior and highlight the importance of personality in understanding individual differences in donations.

这项预先登记的研究评估了对 2023 年 2 月土耳其和叙利亚地震两组灾民的实际捐款情况,同时考虑了作为捐款决定因素的各种理论相关背景和个性因素。在一个多样化的德国在线样本(N = 496)中,大多数参与者(62.9%)都捐了款,只有少数人(24.0%)在捐款时有所选择,对一组灾民的捐款多于对另一组灾民的捐款。除了环境因素外,诚实-谦逊的性格也增加了对捐赠行为的预测。然而,选择性捐赠在很大程度上受(情境)需求感的驱动。总之,我们的研究结果提供了对捐赠行为的新见解,并强调了人格在理解捐赠个体差异方面的重要性。
{"title":"Who helps whom in times of crisis? An investigation of actual donations to two groups of earthquake victims","authors":"Büsra Elif Yelbuz,&nbsp;Isabel Thielmann","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104476","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104476","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This pre-registered study assessed actual donations to two groups of victims of the earthquake in Turkey and Syria in February 2023 while considering various theoretically-relevant contextual and personality factors as determinants of donations. In a diverse German online sample (<em>N</em> = 496), most participants (62.9 %) donated something, and only few (24.0 %) were selective in their donations, donating more to one group of victims than to the other. Dispositional honesty-humility added to the prediction of donation behavior beyond contextual factors. Selective donations, however, were largely driven by (contextual) perceptions of need. Overall, our findings provide novel insights into donation behavior and highlight the importance of personality in understanding individual differences in donations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000242/pdfft?md5=360c414b5211d1de1ac65ec460ef9a03&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000242-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139986987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
For whom is mind wandering stressful: The moderating role of dispositional emotionality and personality in predicting emotional experiences in everyday life 思绪游荡会给谁带来压力?倾向性情绪和人格在预测日常生活中的情绪体验中的调节作用
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104472
Matthew J. Zawadzki, Armin Hojjaty, Anna-Celine Guilas, Anna V. Song

This study tested whether dispositional emotionality and personality traits moderated the emotion a person experienced when engaging in mind wandering. Participants (n = 264) completed measures of dispositional emotionality and personality. Then on two separate days they completed a 24-hour ecological momentary assessment protocol, responding every 30 min during wake times if they were mind wandering, and how angry, sad, anxious, and happy they felt, for a total of 8,530 assessments. Using multilevel models, we found the following when mind wandering versus not: trait anger predicted more anger, depressive symptomatology more sadness, openness to experience less sadness, neuroticism more anger, and conscientiousness less happiness. Results indicate a synchrony between emotions experienced while mind wandering to one’s dispositional emotionality and personality.

本研究测试了处置情绪性和人格特质是否会调节一个人在进行思绪游荡时所体验到的情绪。参与者(n = 264)完成了处置情绪性和人格的测量。然后,他们分别在两天内完成了一项 24 小时生态瞬间评估方案,在清醒时每隔 30 分钟回答自己是否在胡思乱想,以及他们感到愤怒、悲伤、焦虑和快乐的程度,总共进行了 8,530 次评估。通过使用多层次模型,我们发现当思想游离与否时,特质愤怒预示着更多的愤怒,抑郁症状预示着更多的悲伤,开放性体验预示着更少的悲伤,神经质预示着更多的愤怒,自觉性预示着更少的快乐。研究结果表明,思维游离时的情绪与个人的倾向性情绪和人格之间具有同步性。
{"title":"For whom is mind wandering stressful: The moderating role of dispositional emotionality and personality in predicting emotional experiences in everyday life","authors":"Matthew J. Zawadzki,&nbsp;Armin Hojjaty,&nbsp;Anna-Celine Guilas,&nbsp;Anna V. Song","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104472","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104472","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study tested whether dispositional emotionality and personality traits moderated the emotion a person experienced when engaging in mind wandering. Participants (<em>n</em> = 264) completed measures of dispositional emotionality and personality. Then on two separate days they completed a 24-hour ecological momentary assessment protocol, responding every 30 min during wake times if they were mind wandering, and how angry, sad, anxious, and happy they felt, for a total of 8,530 assessments. Using multilevel models, we found the following when mind wandering versus not: trait anger predicted more anger, depressive symptomatology more sadness, openness to experience less sadness, neuroticism more anger, and conscientiousness less happiness. Results indicate a synchrony between emotions experienced while mind wandering to one’s dispositional emotionality and personality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000205/pdfft?md5=49bf68130bc730df1b3a92aa84873c73&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000205-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139914590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Research in Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1