首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Research in Personality最新文献

英文 中文
Personality and conceptions of religiosity across the world’s religions 世界各宗教的个性和宗教观念
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496
Erica Baranski , Gwendolyn Gardiner , Nicholas Shaman , Jennah Shagan , Daniel Lee , David Funder , Members of the International Situations Project

Research assessing personality traits and religiosity across cultures has typically neglected variation across religious affiliations and has been limited to a small number of personality traits. This study examines the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and their facets, two theoretically distinct measures of religiosity, and twelve other personality traits across seven religious affiliations and 61 countries/regions. The proportion of participants following a religion varied substantially across countries (e.g., Indonesia = 99%; Estonia = 7%). Both measures of religiosity were related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, happiness, and fairness; however; relations with religiosity as a social axiom were stronger and less variable across religious affiliations. Additionally, personality-religiosity links were more robust in low-development, high-conflict, and collectivist nations.

对不同文化背景下的人格特质和宗教信仰进行评估的研究通常忽视了不同宗教信仰之间的差异,而且仅限于少数人格特质。本研究探讨了大五人格特质及其侧面、两种理论上不同的宗教性测量方法以及其他十二种人格特质在七个宗教派别和 61 个国家/地区之间的关系。不同国家的参与者信奉宗教的比例差异很大(例如,印度尼西亚=99%;爱沙尼亚=7%)。宗教信仰的两个测量指标都与宜人性、自觉性、幸福感和公平性相关;但是,宗教信仰作为一种社会公理,与不同宗教信仰之间的关系更为紧密,且变化较少。此外,在低发展水平、高冲突和集体主义国家,个性与宗教信仰的联系更为紧密。
{"title":"Personality and conceptions of religiosity across the world’s religions","authors":"Erica Baranski ,&nbsp;Gwendolyn Gardiner ,&nbsp;Nicholas Shaman ,&nbsp;Jennah Shagan ,&nbsp;Daniel Lee ,&nbsp;David Funder ,&nbsp;Members of the International Situations Project","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research assessing personality traits and religiosity across cultures has typically neglected variation across religious affiliations and has been limited to a small number of personality traits. This study examines the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and their facets, two theoretically distinct measures of religiosity, and twelve other personality traits across seven religious affiliations and 61 countries/regions. The proportion of participants following a religion varied substantially across countries (e.g., Indonesia = 99%; Estonia = 7%). Both measures of religiosity were related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, happiness, and fairness; however; relations with religiosity as a social axiom were stronger and less variable across religious affiliations. Additionally, personality-religiosity links were more robust in low-development, high-conflict, and collectivist nations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104496"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141023348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Agency and communion ratings do not suggest shifting gender norms in American Society: A registered report 机构和圣餐评级并不表明美国社会的性别规范发生了变化:注册报告
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493
Azriel Grysman , Jordan A. Booker

Gender role scales measure concepts related to agency and communion as masculine- and feminine-typed traits. However, previous findings showed that 18-to-29-year-old women and men did not differ on a scale of communion, whereas 30–40-year-old women and men did. This study attempted to replicate these findings and to expand to a lifespan sample 10 years after initial data collection to clarify whether interactions by age support developmental and/or cohort-related trends for gender and communion. Results contradict a cohort explanation, and in fact show a weaker interaction between age and gender than prior studies. We conclude that mean gender differences are broadly stable across the lifespan, though they are somewhat weaker in emerging adult years, suggesting a small developmental effect.

性别角色量表测量的是作为男性和女性特质的代理和共融相关概念。然而,之前的研究结果表明,18 至 29 岁的女性和男性在 "交流 "量表上没有差异,而 30 至 40 岁的女性和男性在 "交流 "量表上有差异。本研究试图复制这些研究结果,并在初始数据收集 10 年后将研究扩展到终身样本,以澄清年龄的相互作用是否支持性别和共融的发展和/或与队列相关的趋势。研究结果与队列的解释相矛盾,事实上,与之前的研究相比,年龄与性别之间的交互作用更弱。我们得出的结论是,性别差异的平均值在整个生命周期中大致稳定,但在成年后会有所减弱,这表明发展效应较小。
{"title":"Agency and communion ratings do not suggest shifting gender norms in American Society: A registered report","authors":"Azriel Grysman ,&nbsp;Jordan A. Booker","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Gender role scales measure concepts related to agency and communion as masculine- and feminine-typed traits. However, previous findings showed that 18-to-29-year-old women and men did not differ on a scale of communion, whereas 30–40-year-old women and men did. This study attempted to replicate these findings and to expand to a lifespan sample 10 years after initial data collection to clarify whether interactions by age support developmental and/or cohort-related trends for gender and communion. Results contradict a cohort explanation, and in fact show a weaker interaction between age and gender than prior studies. We conclude that mean gender differences are broadly stable across the lifespan, though they are somewhat weaker in emerging adult years, suggesting a small developmental effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104493"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140822898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality expression in body motion dynamics: An enactive, embodied, and complex systems perspective 身体运动动力学中的个性表达:主动、体现和复杂系统的视角
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495
Nicol A. Arellano-Véliz , Ralf F.A. Cox , Bertus F. Jeronimus , Ramón D. Castillo , E. Saskia Kunnen

We studied body motion dynamics and personality differences using complex systems methods. 105 adults (aged 18–33, 70% women) completed a 15-minute laboratory task covering three self-referencing topics (self-introduction, bodily perception/sensory life, socio-emotional life). Body motion dynamics were extracted from videos using a frame-by-frame differentiation method. Recurrence Quantification Analysis derived the measures of Determinism, Entropy, Laminarity, and Mean-Line. Multilevel models estimated personality (IPIP-NEO-120) and situational effects. Neuroticism predicted lower determinism and fluctuating dynamics in bodily perception and socioemotional life; less complexity and stability during socioemotional topics, and increased negative affect. Extraversion predicted regular/deterministic dynamics during bodily perception. Conscientiousness predicted lower determinism and increased variability. Agreeableness predicted lower post-task negative affect. Findings are discussed within embodied, enactive, complex systems, and personality frameworks.

我们采用复杂系统方法研究了身体运动动力学和人格差异。105 名成年人(18-33 岁,70% 为女性)完成了一项 15 分钟的实验室任务,该任务涵盖三个自我参照主题(自我介绍、身体感知/感官生活、社会情感生活)。使用逐帧区分法从视频中提取身体运动动态。递归量化分析法得出了确定性、熵、层状性和平均线等指标。多层次模型估计了人格(IPIP-NEO-120)和情境效应。神经质预示着身体感知和社会情感生活中较低的决定性和波动性;社会情感主题中较低的复杂性和稳定性,以及较高的负面情绪。外向性预示着身体感知的规律性/决定性动态。自觉性预示着较低的决定性和较高的可变性。宜人性预示着较低的任务后负面情绪。研究结果将在具身、能动、复杂系统和人格框架内进行讨论。
{"title":"Personality expression in body motion dynamics: An enactive, embodied, and complex systems perspective","authors":"Nicol A. Arellano-Véliz ,&nbsp;Ralf F.A. Cox ,&nbsp;Bertus F. Jeronimus ,&nbsp;Ramón D. Castillo ,&nbsp;E. Saskia Kunnen","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We studied body motion dynamics and personality differences using complex systems methods. 105 adults (aged 18–33, 70% women) completed a 15-minute laboratory task covering three self-referencing topics (self-introduction, bodily perception/sensory life, socio-emotional life). Body motion dynamics were extracted from videos using a frame-by-frame differentiation method. Recurrence Quantification Analysis derived the measures of Determinism, Entropy, Laminarity, and Mean-Line. Multilevel models estimated personality (IPIP-NEO-120) and situational effects. Neuroticism predicted lower determinism and fluctuating dynamics in bodily perception and socioemotional life; less complexity and stability during socioemotional topics, and increased negative affect. Extraversion predicted regular/deterministic dynamics during bodily perception. Conscientiousness predicted lower determinism and increased variability. Agreeableness predicted lower post-task negative affect. Findings are discussed within embodied, enactive, complex systems, and personality frameworks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104495"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000436/pdfft?md5=a033a8c652fbcff52e4e53206537319c&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000436-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comprehensive examination of personality factor and facet associations with daily stress processes 全面研究人格因素和面相与日常压力过程的关系
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494
Paula G. Williams, Steven E. Carlson

Although psychosocial stress is a putative mechanism for personality-health associations, research has been limited by a lack of comprehensive, fine-tuned assessment. In the current study, a large sample (n = 655) of young adults completed facet-level personality assessment followed by a 14-day experience sampling of stress exposure, emotional stress reactivity, pre-sleep arousal, and sleep. Multilevel model analyses confirmed that neuroticism, across facets, was the strongest predictor of daily stress. Facet-level analyses indicated that assertiveness, trust, and self-control were particularly relevant to daily stress. Personality moderated stress exposure associations with pre-sleep arousal (conscientiousness, aesthetic chill proneness) and restorative sleep (openness, aesthetic chill proneness). Findings confirm the significance of personality in daily stress. Future directions and implications for stress intervention are discussed.

尽管社会心理压力是人格与健康之间关联的一种假定机制,但由于缺乏全面、精细的评估,研究一直受到限制。在本研究中,一个大样本(n = 655)的年轻成年人完成了多面性人格评估,然后对压力暴露、情绪压力反应性、睡前唤醒和睡眠进行了为期 14 天的经验采样。多层次模型分析证实,神经质是预测日常压力最大的因素。面层分析表明,自信、信任和自我控制与日常压力特别相关。人格调节了压力暴露与睡前唤醒(自觉性、审美寒冷倾向)和恢复性睡眠(开放性、审美寒冷倾向)之间的关联。研究结果证实了人格在日常压力中的重要性。本文讨论了压力干预的未来方向和影响。
{"title":"A comprehensive examination of personality factor and facet associations with daily stress processes","authors":"Paula G. Williams,&nbsp;Steven E. Carlson","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although psychosocial stress is a putative mechanism for personality-health associations, research has been limited by a lack of comprehensive, fine-tuned assessment. In the current study, a large sample (n = 655) of young adults completed facet-level personality assessment followed by a 14-day experience sampling of stress exposure, emotional stress reactivity, pre-sleep arousal, and sleep. Multilevel model analyses confirmed that neuroticism, across facets, was the strongest predictor of daily stress. Facet-level analyses indicated that assertiveness, trust, and self-control were particularly relevant to daily stress. Personality moderated stress exposure associations with pre-sleep arousal (conscientiousness, aesthetic chill proneness) and restorative sleep (openness, aesthetic chill proneness). Findings confirm the significance of personality in daily stress. Future directions and implications for stress intervention are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104494"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The big five inventory (BFI-2): Psychometric properties and validation in Serbian language 五大清单(BFI-2):心理统计特性和塞尔维亚语验证
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492
Snežana Smederevac , Dušanka Mitrović , Selka Sadiković , Bojana M. Dinić , Oliver P. John , Christopher J. Soto

The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2), as a comprehensive measure encompassing the Big Five domains and their facets, has undergone numerous validations in various languages. To validate a Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2, data from two samples, comprising a total of 1016 adult participants, along with a US sample (N = 1000), were utilized. The originally proposed factor structure of Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2 was confirmed, along with partial scalar invariance across Serbian and US samples. Convergent validity was established in relation to an alternative Big Five measure, as well as six-factor and seven-factor models. Criterion validity was confirmed through correlations with various behavior, cognitive, and emotional indicators. These findings contribute to evidence supporting the cross-cultural validity of BFI-2.

大五量表-2(BFI-2)作为一种涵盖大五领域及其各个方面的综合测量方法,已经用各种语言进行了多次验证。为了验证塞尔维亚语改编的 BFI-2,我们使用了来自两个样本的数据,包括总计 1016 名成年参与者和一个美国样本(N = 1000)。最初提出的 BFI-2 塞尔维亚语改编版的因子结构以及塞尔维亚语和美国样本的部分标度不变性得到了证实。与其他大五测量方法以及六因素和七因素模型相比,确定了收敛效度。通过与各种行为、认知和情绪指标的相关性,确认了标准效度。这些研究结果为支持 BFI-2 的跨文化有效性提供了证据。
{"title":"The big five inventory (BFI-2): Psychometric properties and validation in Serbian language","authors":"Snežana Smederevac ,&nbsp;Dušanka Mitrović ,&nbsp;Selka Sadiković ,&nbsp;Bojana M. Dinić ,&nbsp;Oliver P. John ,&nbsp;Christopher J. Soto","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2), as a comprehensive measure encompassing the Big Five domains and their facets, has undergone numerous validations in various languages. To validate a Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2, data from two samples, comprising a total of 1016 adult participants, along with a US sample (N = 1000), were utilized. The originally proposed factor structure of Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2 was confirmed, along with partial scalar invariance across Serbian and US samples. Convergent validity was established in relation to an alternative Big Five measure, as well as six-factor and seven-factor models. Criterion validity was confirmed through correlations with various behavior, cognitive, and emotional indicators. These findings contribute to evidence supporting the cross-cultural validity of BFI-2.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104492"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140622169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are some people more accurate than others about the unique impressions they make on close others? 对于自己给亲密的人留下的独特印象,有些人是否比其他人更准确?
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491
N. Elsaadawy , E.N. Carlson , P. Borkenau

Some meta-perceivers have more insight than others into the impressions they make, but what is this meta-accuracy about? Do good meta-perceivers have insight into the unique impressions they make (dyadic meta-accuracy), or do they simply understand their reputation (generalized meta-accuracy)? In two studies, wemetaces in dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy among close others, as well as potential mechanisms and correlates. Results suggest that, among close others, there are good meta-perceivers of dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy. Good meta-perceivers of dyadic meta-accuracy form more differentiated meta-perceptions, while good meta-perceivers of generalized meta-accuracy make and think they make consistent impressions that align with their self-views. Thus, among close others – unlike in first impressions – there are good meta-perceivers who perspective-take.

有些元感知者比其他人更能洞察自己给人留下的印象,但这种元准确性是怎么回事呢?优秀的元感知者是能洞察自己给人留下的独特印象(二元准确度),还是仅仅了解自己的声誉(广义准确度)?在两项研究中,我们分析了亲密他人中二元元准确度和广义元准确度的差异,以及潜在的机制和相关因素。研究结果表明,在关系密切的其他人中,有很好的元感知者,他们的元准确性很高。优秀的二元元准确度元感知者会形成更多不同的元感知,而优秀的广义元准确度元感知者则会产生并认为他们会产生与自我观点一致的印象。因此,与第一印象不同的是,在关系密切的人当中,也有善于透视的元感知者。
{"title":"Are some people more accurate than others about the unique impressions they make on close others?","authors":"N. Elsaadawy ,&nbsp;E.N. Carlson ,&nbsp;P. Borkenau","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Some meta-perceivers have more insight than others into the impressions they make, but what is this meta-accuracy about? Do good meta-perceivers have insight into the unique impressions they make (dyadic meta-accuracy), or do they simply understand their reputation (generalized meta-accuracy)? In two studies, wemetaces in dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy among close others, as well as potential mechanisms and correlates. Results suggest that, among close others, there are good meta-perceivers of dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy. Good meta-perceivers of dyadic meta-accuracy form more differentiated meta-perceptions, while good meta-perceivers of generalized meta-accuracy make and think they make consistent impressions that align with their self-views. Thus, among close others – unlike in first impressions – there are good meta-perceivers who perspective-take.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104491"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000394/pdfft?md5=24a3f4a11c8bfe0554c5cffe95e69c2a&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000394-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140618713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intolerance of uncertainty predicts indecisiveness and safety behavior in real-life decision making: Results from an experience sampling study 对不确定性的不容忍度可预测现实决策中的优柔寡断和安全行为:经验取样研究的结果
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490
Helmut Appel , Julia Krasko , Maike Luhmann , Alexander L. Gerlach

In decision making, intolerance of uncertainty (IU) may be linked to safety behavior, which could ironically maintain IU and, thus, foster indecisiveness. In a smartphone-based experience sampling study, 247 participants described their real-life decisions six times per day and rated (a) their situational indecisiveness, (b) decision characteristics, (c) situational IU, and (d) problematic safety behaviors. Participants higher in dispositional IU reported more indecisiveness and engaged in more problematic safety behavior across measurements. The same relationships were observed with situational IU at the level of individual measurements. Engaging in more problematic safety behaviors during the first days predicted indecisiveness on the last day, mediated by IU. The results demonstrate the real-life relevance of the relationship between IU, indecisiveness, and safety behavior.

在决策过程中,对不确定性的不容忍度(IU)可能与安全行为有关,而安全行为可能会具有讽刺意味地维持不确定性,从而助长优柔寡断。在一项基于智能手机的经验取样研究中,247 名参与者每天六次描述他们在现实生活中做出的决定,并对以下方面进行评分:(a)他们的情境优柔寡断性;(b)决策特征;(c)情境 IU;以及(d)有问题的安全行为。倾向性 IU 值较高的受试者在各次测量中表现出更多的优柔寡断,并参与了更多有问题的安全行为。在个人测量水平上,也观察到了与情境性 IU 的相同关系。在前几天参与更多有问题的安全行为可以预测最后一天的优柔寡断,而这是由 IU 调解的。这些结果表明,IU、优柔寡断和安全行为之间的关系与现实生活息息相关。
{"title":"Intolerance of uncertainty predicts indecisiveness and safety behavior in real-life decision making: Results from an experience sampling study","authors":"Helmut Appel ,&nbsp;Julia Krasko ,&nbsp;Maike Luhmann ,&nbsp;Alexander L. Gerlach","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104490","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In decision making, intolerance of uncertainty (IU) may be linked to safety behavior, which could ironically maintain IU and, thus, foster indecisiveness. In a smartphone-based experience sampling study, 247 participants described their real-life decisions six times per day and rated (a) their situational indecisiveness, (b) decision characteristics, (c) situational IU, and (d) problematic safety behaviors. Participants higher in dispositional IU reported more indecisiveness and engaged in more problematic safety behavior across measurements. The same relationships were observed with situational IU at the level of individual measurements. Engaging in more problematic safety behaviors during the first days predicted indecisiveness on the last day, mediated by IU. The results demonstrate the real-life relevance of the relationship between IU, indecisiveness, and safety behavior.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104490"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000382/pdfft?md5=9f092953545ce88d8ebe5c10e351b44f&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000382-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140399112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does narrating the life story predict changes in personality traits and characteristics? 讲述人生故事能否预测人格特质和特征的变化?
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489
Rebekka Weidmann , Janina Larissa Bühler , Jenna Wünsche , Alexander Grob

To measure narratives, individuals often participate in a life story interview, which bears similarity to a narrative therapeutic approach. Given that clinical interventions were shown to impact change in personality traits and characteristics, the present study explored whether narrating one’s life story is also linked to such changes. The pre-registered study compared 123 life-story-interview participants (Mage = 35.44 years) with 123 control-sample participants (Mage = 35.50 years). We assessed participants’ personality traits, optimism, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and affect before and 1 and 3 years after the interview. The findings suggest that the life story interview does not lead to consistent changes in personality traits and characteristics, indicating that this interview setting developed for measurement is different from an intervention.

为了测量叙事,个人通常会参加人生故事访谈,这与叙事治疗方法有相似之处。鉴于临床干预对人格特质和特征的改变有影响,本研究探讨了叙述自己的人生故事是否也与这种改变有关。这项预先登记的研究比较了 123 名生活故事访谈参与者(年龄为 35.44 岁)和 123 名对照样本参与者(年龄为 35.50 岁)。我们在访谈前、访谈后 1 年和 3 年评估了参与者的人格特质、乐观程度、自尊、生活满意度和情感。研究结果表明,生命故事访谈不会导致人格特质和特征发生一致的变化,这表明这种为测量而开发的访谈设置不同于干预措施。
{"title":"Does narrating the life story predict changes in personality traits and characteristics?","authors":"Rebekka Weidmann ,&nbsp;Janina Larissa Bühler ,&nbsp;Jenna Wünsche ,&nbsp;Alexander Grob","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104489","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To measure narratives, individuals often participate in a life story interview, which bears similarity to a narrative therapeutic approach. Given that clinical interventions were shown to impact change in personality traits and characteristics, the present study explored whether narrating one’s life story is also linked to such changes. The pre-registered study compared 123 life-story-interview participants (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> <em>=</em> 35.44 years) with 123 control-sample participants (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> <em>=</em> 35.50 years). We assessed participants’ personality traits, optimism, self-esteem, life satisfaction, and affect before and 1 and 3 years after the interview. The findings suggest that the life story interview does not lead to consistent changes in personality traits and characteristics, indicating that this interview setting developed for measurement is different from an intervention.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104489"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140332976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Further exploring the impact of cumulative lifetime adversity on life satisfaction, psychological flourishing, and depressive symptoms 进一步探讨一生中累积的逆境对生活满意度、心理健康和抑郁症状的影响
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488
Stephanie A. Bossert , Eranda Jayawickreme , Laura E.R. Blackie , Veronica T. Cole

Current research provides conflicting views of the relationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and dispositional functioning. We examined this relationship in a sample of adults (N = 1009), conducting the analysis three ways: by utilizing a sum score of adversity across all ages; examining events that occurred before and after age 18 separately; and assessing this relationship using empirically-derived domains of adversity. Results indicate that the type of adversity has a unique impact on future well-being. These findings underscore that adversity’s unique impact on well-being is influenced by the way in which adversity is operationalized, more so than by the timing of adverse events. Results highlight the importance of clarifying the nuanced relationship between adverse life events and mental health in future research.

目前的研究对一生中累积的逆境与性格功能之间的关系提供了相互矛盾的观点。我们对成人样本(N = 1009)中的这一关系进行了研究,分析方法有三种:利用所有年龄段的逆境总分;分别研究 18 岁之前和之后发生的事件;利用从经验中得出的逆境领域评估这一关系。结果表明,逆境的类型对未来的幸福感有独特的影响。这些发现强调,逆境对幸福感的独特影响受到逆境的操作方式的影响,而不是逆境事件发生的时间。研究结果凸显了在未来研究中阐明逆境与心理健康之间微妙关系的重要性。
{"title":"Further exploring the impact of cumulative lifetime adversity on life satisfaction, psychological flourishing, and depressive symptoms","authors":"Stephanie A. Bossert ,&nbsp;Eranda Jayawickreme ,&nbsp;Laura E.R. Blackie ,&nbsp;Veronica T. Cole","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104488","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Current research provides conflicting views of the relationship between cumulative lifetime adversity and dispositional functioning. We examined this relationship in a sample of adults (<em>N</em> = 1009), conducting the analysis three ways: by utilizing a sum score of adversity across all ages; examining events that occurred before and after age 18 separately; and assessing this relationship using empirically-derived domains of adversity. Results indicate that the type of adversity has a unique impact on future well-being. These findings underscore that adversity’s unique impact on well-being is influenced by the way in which adversity is operationalized, more so than by the timing of adverse events. Results highlight the importance of clarifying the nuanced relationship between adverse life events and mental health in future research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104488"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140282139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characterizing stress processes by linking big five personality states, traits, and day-to-day stressors 将五大人格状态、特质和日常压力源联系起来,描述压力过程
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487
Whitney R. Ringwald , Sienna R. Nielsen , Janan Mostajabi , Colin E. Vize , Tessa van den Berg , Stephen B. Manuck , Anna L. Marsland , Aidan G.C. Wright

The accumulation of day-to-day stressors can impact mental and physical health. How people respond to stressful events is a key mechanism responsible for the effects of stress, and individual differences in stress responses can either perpetuate or prevent negative consequences. Most research on daily stress processes has focused on affective responses to stressors, but stress responses can involve more than just affect (e.g., behavior, cognitions). Additionally, most research has studied the role of neuroticism in shaping those responses, but many other individual differences are associated with stress. In this study, we more broadly characterized daily stress processes by expanding the nomological networks of stress responses to include Big Five personality states. We also linked those stress responses to all Big Five traits, as well as individual differences in stress variety, severity, and controllability. We studied a sample of participants (N = 1,090) who reported on stressful events, their appraisal of events in terms of severity and controllability, and their Big Five personality states daily for 8–10 days (N = 8,870 observations). Multi-level structural equation models were used to separate how characteristics of the perceived stressful situation and characteristics of the person play into daily stress processes. Results showed that (1) all Big Five personality states shift in response to perceived stress, (2) all Big Five personality traits relate to average levels of perceived stress variety, severity, and controllability, (3) individual differences in personality and average perceived stress variety and perceived severity relate to the strength of personality state responses to daily stress, albeit in a more limited fashion. Our results point to new pathways by which stressors affect people in everyday life and begin to clarify processes that may explain individual differences in risk or resilience to the harmful effects of stress.

日常压力的积累会影响身心健康。人们如何应对压力事件是造成压力影响的一个关键机制,而压力反应的个体差异可以延续或防止负面影响。大多数有关日常压力过程的研究都侧重于对压力源的情感反应,但压力反应可能不仅仅涉及情感(如行为、认知)。此外,大多数研究都研究了神经质在形成这些反应中的作用,但许多其他个体差异也与压力有关。在本研究中,我们通过扩展压力反应的名义网络,将大五人格状态纳入其中,从而更广泛地描述了日常压力过程。我们还将这些压力反应与所有五大性格特征以及压力种类、严重程度和可控性方面的个体差异联系起来。我们对参与者样本(N = 1,090)进行了研究,这些参与者在 8-10 天内每天报告压力事件、他们对事件严重性和可控性的评价以及他们的大五人格状态(N = 8,870 个观察值)。我们使用多层次结构方程模型来区分感知到的压力情境特征和个人特征在日常压力过程中的作用。结果表明:(1) 所有的大五人格状态都会对感知到的压力做出反应,(2) 所有的大五人格特质都与感知到的压力种类、严重程度和可控性的平均水平有关,(3) 个体的人格差异以及平均感知到的压力种类和严重程度与人格状态对日常压力反应的强度有关,尽管这种关系比较有限。我们的研究结果指出了压力因素在日常生活中对人们产生影响的新途径,并开始阐明可能解释个体差异对压力有害影响的风险或复原力的过程。
{"title":"Characterizing stress processes by linking big five personality states, traits, and day-to-day stressors","authors":"Whitney R. Ringwald ,&nbsp;Sienna R. Nielsen ,&nbsp;Janan Mostajabi ,&nbsp;Colin E. Vize ,&nbsp;Tessa van den Berg ,&nbsp;Stephen B. Manuck ,&nbsp;Anna L. Marsland ,&nbsp;Aidan G.C. Wright","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104487","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The accumulation of day-to-day stressors can impact mental and physical health. How people respond to stressful events is a key mechanism responsible for the effects of stress, and individual differences in stress responses can either perpetuate or prevent negative consequences. Most research on daily stress processes has focused on affective responses to stressors, but stress responses can involve more than just affect (e.g., behavior, cognitions). Additionally, most research has studied the role of neuroticism in shaping those responses, but many other individual differences are associated with stress. In this study, we more broadly characterized daily stress processes by expanding the nomological networks of stress responses to include Big Five personality states. We also linked those stress responses to all Big Five traits, as well as individual differences in stress variety, severity, and controllability. We studied a sample of participants (<em>N</em> = 1,090) who reported on stressful events, their appraisal of events in terms of severity and controllability, and their Big Five personality states daily for 8–10 days (<em>N</em> = 8,870 observations). Multi-level structural equation models were used to separate how characteristics of the perceived stressful situation and characteristics of the person play into daily stress processes. Results showed that (1) all Big Five personality states shift in response to perceived stress, (2) all Big Five personality traits relate to average levels of perceived stress variety, severity, and controllability, (3) individual differences in personality and average perceived stress variety and perceived severity relate to the strength of personality state responses to daily stress, albeit in a more limited fashion. Our results point to new pathways by which stressors affect people in everyday life and begin to clarify processes that may explain individual differences in risk or resilience to the harmful effects of stress.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 104487"},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140162695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Research in Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1