首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Research in Personality最新文献

英文 中文
The autobiographical critic within: Perceiving oneself as a major character in one’s life story predicts well-being 内心的自传评论家将自己视为人生故事中的主要角色可预测幸福感
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104510
Ryan Goffredi , Kennon M. Sheldon

Narrative identity research typically assumes that people always play the role of the main character in the life stories they provide (McAdams, 2018). However, it is possible that some people view themselves as playing the role of a “side” character or minor character in their life story. Such views of the self are likely to influence well-being outcomes. In three studies we use a novel self-report method to show that seeing oneself as a major versus minor character within one’s own life story significantly impacts well-being both prospectively and retrospectively. Additionally, we demonstrate that this major character construct is associated with rated psychological need satisfaction, autonomous goal pursuit, and coded agency. We believe these findings contribute to expanding available autobiographical assessments and predictions of well-being from narrative data.

叙事身份研究通常假定,人们总是在自己提供的人生故事中扮演主角(McAdams,2018)。然而,有些人可能认为自己在人生故事中扮演的是 "配角 "或次要角色。这种自我观很可能会影响幸福感的结果。在三项研究中,我们使用了一种新颖的自我报告方法,证明在自己的人生故事中,将自己视为主要角色与次要角色会对幸福感产生显著的影响,无论是前瞻性影响还是回顾性影响。此外,我们还证明,这种主要角色的构建与额定心理需求满足度、自主目标追求和编码代理有关。我们相信,这些发现有助于扩大现有的自传性评估以及从叙事数据中预测幸福感。
{"title":"The autobiographical critic within: Perceiving oneself as a major character in one’s life story predicts well-being","authors":"Ryan Goffredi ,&nbsp;Kennon M. Sheldon","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104510","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104510","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Narrative identity research typically assumes that people always play the role of the main character in the life stories they provide (<span>McAdams, 2018</span>). However, it is possible that some people view themselves as playing the role of a “side” character or minor character in their life story. Such views of the self are likely to influence well-being outcomes. In three studies we use a novel self-report method to show that seeing oneself as a major versus minor character within one’s own life story significantly impacts well-being both prospectively and retrospectively. Additionally, we demonstrate that this major character construct is associated with rated psychological need satisfaction, autonomous goal pursuit, and coded agency. We believe these findings contribute to expanding available autobiographical assessments and predictions of well-being from narrative data.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141404788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do religious practices improve one’s mood? Daily religious behaviors are associated with daily wellbeing and buffer against hassles and uplifts 日常宗教行为与日常幸福感有关,并能缓冲烦恼和振奋精神
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104499
Crystal L. Park , Joshua A. Wilt

Effects of trait religiousness on wellbeing are well-documented, but the potential of daily religious practices to influence daily wellbeing and mitigate the commonplace ups and downs of daily life has rarely been examined. This study explored how trait religiousness and daily religious behaviors relate to day-to-day wellbeing (i.e., positive and negative affect), especially in the context of daily stresses and uplifts. A national US sample of 184 adults completed baseline measures and 14 daily surveys. Multilevel modeling revealed the relevance of daily religious behaviors (rather than trait religiousness) for daily affect. In general, daily religious behaviors showed salutary main effects on daily affect and buffered associations of daily stresses and uplifts on affect. In contrast, trait religiousness showed weak or null main effects and did not buffer associations between stresses/uplifts and affect. Notably, some associations between daily religious behaviors and affect were stronger for individuals higher in trait religiousness. Item-level analyses showed that most religious behaviors had similar associations as composite daily religious behaviors in predicting positive affect, whereas only specific daily religious behaviors predicted negative affect. Findings suggest future research might profitably attend to daily religious behaviors to further understand how religiousness influences emotional wellbeing.

特质宗教性对幸福感的影响已被充分证明,但日常宗教实践对影响日常生活幸福感和缓解日常生活中常见的起伏的潜力却鲜有研究。本研究探讨了特质宗教性和日常宗教行为与日常幸福感(即积极和消极情绪)之间的关系,尤其是在日常压力和振奋的背景下。一个由 184 名成年人组成的美国全国样本完成了基线测量和 14 项日常调查。多层次模型揭示了日常宗教行为(而非特质宗教性)与日常情绪的相关性。一般来说,日常宗教行为对日常情感有有益的主效应,并能缓冲日常压力和振奋对情感的影响。与此相反,特质宗教性的主效应较弱或为零,并且没有缓冲压力/振奋与情感之间的关联。值得注意的是,对于特质宗教性较高的个体来说,日常宗教行为与情感之间的某些关联更为强烈。项目层面的分析表明,在预测积极情绪方面,大多数宗教行为与综合日常宗教行为有相似的关联,而只有特定的日常宗教行为能预测消极情绪。研究结果表明,未来的研究可能会关注日常宗教行为,以进一步了解宗教信仰如何影响情绪健康。
{"title":"Do religious practices improve one’s mood? Daily religious behaviors are associated with daily wellbeing and buffer against hassles and uplifts","authors":"Crystal L. Park ,&nbsp;Joshua A. Wilt","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104499","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104499","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Effects of trait religiousness on wellbeing are well-documented, but the potential of <em>daily</em> religious practices to influence <em>daily</em> wellbeing and mitigate the commonplace ups and downs of daily life has rarely been examined. This study explored how trait religiousness and daily religious behaviors relate to day-to-day wellbeing (i.e., positive and negative affect), especially in the context of daily stresses and uplifts. A national US sample of 184 adults completed baseline measures and 14 daily surveys. Multilevel modeling revealed the relevance of daily religious behaviors (rather than trait religiousness) for daily affect. In general, daily religious behaviors showed salutary main effects on daily affect and buffered associations of daily stresses and uplifts on affect. In contrast, trait religiousness showed weak or null main effects and did not buffer associations between stresses/uplifts and affect. Notably, some associations between daily religious behaviors and affect were stronger for individuals higher in trait religiousness. Item-level analyses showed that most religious behaviors had similar associations as composite daily religious behaviors in predicting positive affect, whereas only specific daily religious behaviors predicted negative affect. Findings suggest future research might profitably attend to daily religious behaviors to further understand how religiousness influences emotional wellbeing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141137656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predicting others’ happiness from brief personal memories 从简短的个人记忆中预测他人的幸福感
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104498
Anurada U. Amarasekera , Tiara A. Cash , Scarlett R.C. MacPherson , Lauren J. Human , Lara B. Aknin

Research suggests that people can predict others’ personality and emotions through various channels, but it is unknown whether strangers can accurately predict a target’s trait happiness from a brief written personal memory, and whether some content is more useful than others. Given the importance of understanding others, we conducted two pre-registered investigations of whether third-party readers could estimate the happiness of 5,169 targets who wrote a brief (average words = 61) personal or prosocial spending memory. We detected a small positive correlation between targets’ trait happiness and readers’ ratings in both conditions, with a larger correlation often found in the personal condition. We then conducted exploratory analyses to probe why certain spending memories facilitate greater trait happiness accuracy by examining word usage.

研究表明,人们可以通过各种渠道预测他人的性格和情绪,但陌生人是否能从简短的书面个人记忆中准确预测目标的特质幸福感,以及某些内容是否比其他内容更有用,这些都是未知数。鉴于了解他人的重要性,我们进行了两项预先登记的调查,研究第三方读者是否能估计出 5169 名撰写了简短(平均字数 = 61)个人或亲社会消费记忆的目标人物的幸福感。我们发现,在这两种情况下,目标人物的特质幸福感与读者的评分之间都存在微小的正相关关系,而在个人情况下,相关性往往更大。然后,我们进行了探索性分析,通过研究词语用法来探究为什么某些消费记忆有助于提高特质幸福感的准确性。
{"title":"Predicting others’ happiness from brief personal memories","authors":"Anurada U. Amarasekera ,&nbsp;Tiara A. Cash ,&nbsp;Scarlett R.C. MacPherson ,&nbsp;Lauren J. Human ,&nbsp;Lara B. Aknin","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104498","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104498","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research suggests that people can predict others’ personality and emotions through various channels, but it is unknown whether strangers can accurately predict a target’s trait happiness from a brief written personal memory, and whether some content is more useful than others. Given the importance of understanding others, we conducted two pre-registered investigations of whether third-party readers could estimate the happiness of 5,169 targets who wrote a brief (average words = 61) personal or prosocial spending memory. We detected a small positive correlation between targets’ trait happiness and readers’ ratings in both conditions, with a larger correlation often found in the personal condition. We then conducted exploratory analyses to probe why certain spending memories facilitate greater trait happiness accuracy by examining word usage.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000461/pdfft?md5=63e95ea1a7f9c9739da227ddce549ddf&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000461-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141038599","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality and individual differences in the relationship between hedonic and eudaimonic motives and well-being in daily life 日常生活中享乐动机和幸福动机与幸福感关系中的人格和个体差异
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104497
Sjoerd van Halem , Eeske van Roekel , Jaap Denissen

To understand the role of personality in the relationship between hedonic motives (e.g., pleasure), eudaimonic motives (e.g., excellence/meaning), and subjective well-being, we sampled 218 university students who completed an online questionnaire and a week of experience sampling surveys. Besides documenting the associations between personality and both motives, we found that the impact of both motives on average subjective well-being did not differ across different levels of the Big Five personality traits. Adding to these trait-level findings, we found that people high on neuroticism generally had more negative experiences when they did not engage in either motive. People high on neuroticism may have a lower setpoint of well-being compared to their peers but may equally benefit from engaging in either motive.

为了了解人格在享乐动机(如快乐)、价值动机(如卓越/意义)与主观幸福感之间的关系中的作用,我们对 218 名大学生进行了抽样调查,他们完成了一份在线问卷和为期一周的经验抽样调查。除了记录人格与这两种动机之间的关联外,我们还发现这两种动机对平均主观幸福感的影响在大五人格特质的不同层次上并无差异。除了这些特质层面的发现之外,我们还发现,神经质高的人在不参与这两种动机时,通常会有更多的负面体验。与同龄人相比,神经质程度高的人的幸福感设定值可能较低,但他们同样可以从参与两种动机中获益。
{"title":"Personality and individual differences in the relationship between hedonic and eudaimonic motives and well-being in daily life","authors":"Sjoerd van Halem ,&nbsp;Eeske van Roekel ,&nbsp;Jaap Denissen","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104497","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104497","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To understand the role of personality in the relationship between hedonic motives (e.g., pleasure), eudaimonic motives (e.g., excellence/meaning), and subjective well-being, we sampled 218 university students who completed an online questionnaire and a week of experience sampling surveys. Besides documenting the associations between personality and both motives, we found that the impact of both motives on average subjective well-being did not differ across different levels of the Big Five personality traits. Adding to these trait-level findings, we found that people high on neuroticism generally had more negative experiences when they did not engage in either motive. People high on neuroticism may have a lower setpoint of well-being compared to their peers but may equally benefit from engaging in either motive.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S009265662400045X/pdfft?md5=806a77b354bddd41138c853c3420b3f7&pid=1-s2.0-S009265662400045X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141027830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality and conceptions of religiosity across the world’s religions 世界各宗教的个性和宗教观念
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496
Erica Baranski , Gwendolyn Gardiner , Nicholas Shaman , Jennah Shagan , Daniel Lee , David Funder , Members of the International Situations Project

Research assessing personality traits and religiosity across cultures has typically neglected variation across religious affiliations and has been limited to a small number of personality traits. This study examines the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and their facets, two theoretically distinct measures of religiosity, and twelve other personality traits across seven religious affiliations and 61 countries/regions. The proportion of participants following a religion varied substantially across countries (e.g., Indonesia = 99%; Estonia = 7%). Both measures of religiosity were related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, happiness, and fairness; however; relations with religiosity as a social axiom were stronger and less variable across religious affiliations. Additionally, personality-religiosity links were more robust in low-development, high-conflict, and collectivist nations.

对不同文化背景下的人格特质和宗教信仰进行评估的研究通常忽视了不同宗教信仰之间的差异,而且仅限于少数人格特质。本研究探讨了大五人格特质及其侧面、两种理论上不同的宗教性测量方法以及其他十二种人格特质在七个宗教派别和 61 个国家/地区之间的关系。不同国家的参与者信奉宗教的比例差异很大(例如,印度尼西亚=99%;爱沙尼亚=7%)。宗教信仰的两个测量指标都与宜人性、自觉性、幸福感和公平性相关;但是,宗教信仰作为一种社会公理,与不同宗教信仰之间的关系更为紧密,且变化较少。此外,在低发展水平、高冲突和集体主义国家,个性与宗教信仰的联系更为紧密。
{"title":"Personality and conceptions of religiosity across the world’s religions","authors":"Erica Baranski ,&nbsp;Gwendolyn Gardiner ,&nbsp;Nicholas Shaman ,&nbsp;Jennah Shagan ,&nbsp;Daniel Lee ,&nbsp;David Funder ,&nbsp;Members of the International Situations Project","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104496","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Research assessing personality traits and religiosity across cultures has typically neglected variation across religious affiliations and has been limited to a small number of personality traits. This study examines the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and their facets, two theoretically distinct measures of religiosity, and twelve other personality traits across seven religious affiliations and 61 countries/regions. The proportion of participants following a religion varied substantially across countries (e.g., Indonesia = 99%; Estonia = 7%). Both measures of religiosity were related to agreeableness, conscientiousness, happiness, and fairness; however; relations with religiosity as a social axiom were stronger and less variable across religious affiliations. Additionally, personality-religiosity links were more robust in low-development, high-conflict, and collectivist nations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141023348","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Agency and communion ratings do not suggest shifting gender norms in American Society: A registered report 机构和圣餐评级并不表明美国社会的性别规范发生了变化:注册报告
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493
Azriel Grysman , Jordan A. Booker

Gender role scales measure concepts related to agency and communion as masculine- and feminine-typed traits. However, previous findings showed that 18-to-29-year-old women and men did not differ on a scale of communion, whereas 30–40-year-old women and men did. This study attempted to replicate these findings and to expand to a lifespan sample 10 years after initial data collection to clarify whether interactions by age support developmental and/or cohort-related trends for gender and communion. Results contradict a cohort explanation, and in fact show a weaker interaction between age and gender than prior studies. We conclude that mean gender differences are broadly stable across the lifespan, though they are somewhat weaker in emerging adult years, suggesting a small developmental effect.

性别角色量表测量的是作为男性和女性特质的代理和共融相关概念。然而,之前的研究结果表明,18 至 29 岁的女性和男性在 "交流 "量表上没有差异,而 30 至 40 岁的女性和男性在 "交流 "量表上有差异。本研究试图复制这些研究结果,并在初始数据收集 10 年后将研究扩展到终身样本,以澄清年龄的相互作用是否支持性别和共融的发展和/或与队列相关的趋势。研究结果与队列的解释相矛盾,事实上,与之前的研究相比,年龄与性别之间的交互作用更弱。我们得出的结论是,性别差异的平均值在整个生命周期中大致稳定,但在成年后会有所减弱,这表明发展效应较小。
{"title":"Agency and communion ratings do not suggest shifting gender norms in American Society: A registered report","authors":"Azriel Grysman ,&nbsp;Jordan A. Booker","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104493","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Gender role scales measure concepts related to agency and communion as masculine- and feminine-typed traits. However, previous findings showed that 18-to-29-year-old women and men did not differ on a scale of communion, whereas 30–40-year-old women and men did. This study attempted to replicate these findings and to expand to a lifespan sample 10 years after initial data collection to clarify whether interactions by age support developmental and/or cohort-related trends for gender and communion. Results contradict a cohort explanation, and in fact show a weaker interaction between age and gender than prior studies. We conclude that mean gender differences are broadly stable across the lifespan, though they are somewhat weaker in emerging adult years, suggesting a small developmental effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140822898","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality expression in body motion dynamics: An enactive, embodied, and complex systems perspective 身体运动动力学中的个性表达:主动、体现和复杂系统的视角
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495
Nicol A. Arellano-Véliz , Ralf F.A. Cox , Bertus F. Jeronimus , Ramón D. Castillo , E. Saskia Kunnen

We studied body motion dynamics and personality differences using complex systems methods. 105 adults (aged 18–33, 70% women) completed a 15-minute laboratory task covering three self-referencing topics (self-introduction, bodily perception/sensory life, socio-emotional life). Body motion dynamics were extracted from videos using a frame-by-frame differentiation method. Recurrence Quantification Analysis derived the measures of Determinism, Entropy, Laminarity, and Mean-Line. Multilevel models estimated personality (IPIP-NEO-120) and situational effects. Neuroticism predicted lower determinism and fluctuating dynamics in bodily perception and socioemotional life; less complexity and stability during socioemotional topics, and increased negative affect. Extraversion predicted regular/deterministic dynamics during bodily perception. Conscientiousness predicted lower determinism and increased variability. Agreeableness predicted lower post-task negative affect. Findings are discussed within embodied, enactive, complex systems, and personality frameworks.

我们采用复杂系统方法研究了身体运动动力学和人格差异。105 名成年人(18-33 岁,70% 为女性)完成了一项 15 分钟的实验室任务,该任务涵盖三个自我参照主题(自我介绍、身体感知/感官生活、社会情感生活)。使用逐帧区分法从视频中提取身体运动动态。递归量化分析法得出了确定性、熵、层状性和平均线等指标。多层次模型估计了人格(IPIP-NEO-120)和情境效应。神经质预示着身体感知和社会情感生活中较低的决定性和波动性;社会情感主题中较低的复杂性和稳定性,以及较高的负面情绪。外向性预示着身体感知的规律性/决定性动态。自觉性预示着较低的决定性和较高的可变性。宜人性预示着较低的任务后负面情绪。研究结果将在具身、能动、复杂系统和人格框架内进行讨论。
{"title":"Personality expression in body motion dynamics: An enactive, embodied, and complex systems perspective","authors":"Nicol A. Arellano-Véliz ,&nbsp;Ralf F.A. Cox ,&nbsp;Bertus F. Jeronimus ,&nbsp;Ramón D. Castillo ,&nbsp;E. Saskia Kunnen","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104495","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We studied body motion dynamics and personality differences using complex systems methods. 105 adults (aged 18–33, 70% women) completed a 15-minute laboratory task covering three self-referencing topics (self-introduction, bodily perception/sensory life, socio-emotional life). Body motion dynamics were extracted from videos using a frame-by-frame differentiation method. Recurrence Quantification Analysis derived the measures of Determinism, Entropy, Laminarity, and Mean-Line. Multilevel models estimated personality (IPIP-NEO-120) and situational effects. Neuroticism predicted lower determinism and fluctuating dynamics in bodily perception and socioemotional life; less complexity and stability during socioemotional topics, and increased negative affect. Extraversion predicted regular/deterministic dynamics during bodily perception. Conscientiousness predicted lower determinism and increased variability. Agreeableness predicted lower post-task negative affect. Findings are discussed within embodied, enactive, complex systems, and personality frameworks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000436/pdfft?md5=a033a8c652fbcff52e4e53206537319c&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000436-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645456","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comprehensive examination of personality factor and facet associations with daily stress processes 全面研究人格因素和面相与日常压力过程的关系
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494
Paula G. Williams, Steven E. Carlson

Although psychosocial stress is a putative mechanism for personality-health associations, research has been limited by a lack of comprehensive, fine-tuned assessment. In the current study, a large sample (n = 655) of young adults completed facet-level personality assessment followed by a 14-day experience sampling of stress exposure, emotional stress reactivity, pre-sleep arousal, and sleep. Multilevel model analyses confirmed that neuroticism, across facets, was the strongest predictor of daily stress. Facet-level analyses indicated that assertiveness, trust, and self-control were particularly relevant to daily stress. Personality moderated stress exposure associations with pre-sleep arousal (conscientiousness, aesthetic chill proneness) and restorative sleep (openness, aesthetic chill proneness). Findings confirm the significance of personality in daily stress. Future directions and implications for stress intervention are discussed.

尽管社会心理压力是人格与健康之间关联的一种假定机制,但由于缺乏全面、精细的评估,研究一直受到限制。在本研究中,一个大样本(n = 655)的年轻成年人完成了多面性人格评估,然后对压力暴露、情绪压力反应性、睡前唤醒和睡眠进行了为期 14 天的经验采样。多层次模型分析证实,神经质是预测日常压力最大的因素。面层分析表明,自信、信任和自我控制与日常压力特别相关。人格调节了压力暴露与睡前唤醒(自觉性、审美寒冷倾向)和恢复性睡眠(开放性、审美寒冷倾向)之间的关联。研究结果证实了人格在日常压力中的重要性。本文讨论了压力干预的未来方向和影响。
{"title":"A comprehensive examination of personality factor and facet associations with daily stress processes","authors":"Paula G. Williams,&nbsp;Steven E. Carlson","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104494","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although psychosocial stress is a putative mechanism for personality-health associations, research has been limited by a lack of comprehensive, fine-tuned assessment. In the current study, a large sample (n = 655) of young adults completed facet-level personality assessment followed by a 14-day experience sampling of stress exposure, emotional stress reactivity, pre-sleep arousal, and sleep. Multilevel model analyses confirmed that neuroticism, across facets, was the strongest predictor of daily stress. Facet-level analyses indicated that assertiveness, trust, and self-control were particularly relevant to daily stress. Personality moderated stress exposure associations with pre-sleep arousal (conscientiousness, aesthetic chill proneness) and restorative sleep (openness, aesthetic chill proneness). Findings confirm the significance of personality in daily stress. Future directions and implications for stress intervention are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The big five inventory (BFI-2): Psychometric properties and validation in Serbian language 五大清单(BFI-2):心理统计特性和塞尔维亚语验证
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492
Snežana Smederevac , Dušanka Mitrović , Selka Sadiković , Bojana M. Dinić , Oliver P. John , Christopher J. Soto

The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2), as a comprehensive measure encompassing the Big Five domains and their facets, has undergone numerous validations in various languages. To validate a Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2, data from two samples, comprising a total of 1016 adult participants, along with a US sample (N = 1000), were utilized. The originally proposed factor structure of Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2 was confirmed, along with partial scalar invariance across Serbian and US samples. Convergent validity was established in relation to an alternative Big Five measure, as well as six-factor and seven-factor models. Criterion validity was confirmed through correlations with various behavior, cognitive, and emotional indicators. These findings contribute to evidence supporting the cross-cultural validity of BFI-2.

大五量表-2(BFI-2)作为一种涵盖大五领域及其各个方面的综合测量方法,已经用各种语言进行了多次验证。为了验证塞尔维亚语改编的 BFI-2,我们使用了来自两个样本的数据,包括总计 1016 名成年参与者和一个美国样本(N = 1000)。最初提出的 BFI-2 塞尔维亚语改编版的因子结构以及塞尔维亚语和美国样本的部分标度不变性得到了证实。与其他大五测量方法以及六因素和七因素模型相比,确定了收敛效度。通过与各种行为、认知和情绪指标的相关性,确认了标准效度。这些研究结果为支持 BFI-2 的跨文化有效性提供了证据。
{"title":"The big five inventory (BFI-2): Psychometric properties and validation in Serbian language","authors":"Snežana Smederevac ,&nbsp;Dušanka Mitrović ,&nbsp;Selka Sadiković ,&nbsp;Bojana M. Dinić ,&nbsp;Oliver P. John ,&nbsp;Christopher J. Soto","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104492","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The Big Five Inventory-2 (BFI-2), as a comprehensive measure encompassing the Big Five domains and their facets, has undergone numerous validations in various languages. To validate a Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2, data from two samples, comprising a total of 1016 adult participants, along with a US sample (N = 1000), were utilized. The originally proposed factor structure of Serbian adaptation of the BFI-2 was confirmed, along with partial scalar invariance across Serbian and US samples. Convergent validity was established in relation to an alternative Big Five measure, as well as six-factor and seven-factor models. Criterion validity was confirmed through correlations with various behavior, cognitive, and emotional indicators. These findings contribute to evidence supporting the cross-cultural validity of BFI-2.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140622169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are some people more accurate than others about the unique impressions they make on close others? 对于自己给亲密的人留下的独特印象,有些人是否比其他人更准确?
IF 3.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491
N. Elsaadawy , E.N. Carlson , P. Borkenau

Some meta-perceivers have more insight than others into the impressions they make, but what is this meta-accuracy about? Do good meta-perceivers have insight into the unique impressions they make (dyadic meta-accuracy), or do they simply understand their reputation (generalized meta-accuracy)? In two studies, wemetaces in dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy among close others, as well as potential mechanisms and correlates. Results suggest that, among close others, there are good meta-perceivers of dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy. Good meta-perceivers of dyadic meta-accuracy form more differentiated meta-perceptions, while good meta-perceivers of generalized meta-accuracy make and think they make consistent impressions that align with their self-views. Thus, among close others – unlike in first impressions – there are good meta-perceivers who perspective-take.

有些元感知者比其他人更能洞察自己给人留下的印象,但这种元准确性是怎么回事呢?优秀的元感知者是能洞察自己给人留下的独特印象(二元准确度),还是仅仅了解自己的声誉(广义准确度)?在两项研究中,我们分析了亲密他人中二元元准确度和广义元准确度的差异,以及潜在的机制和相关因素。研究结果表明,在关系密切的其他人中,有很好的元感知者,他们的元准确性很高。优秀的二元元准确度元感知者会形成更多不同的元感知,而优秀的广义元准确度元感知者则会产生并认为他们会产生与自我观点一致的印象。因此,与第一印象不同的是,在关系密切的人当中,也有善于透视的元感知者。
{"title":"Are some people more accurate than others about the unique impressions they make on close others?","authors":"N. Elsaadawy ,&nbsp;E.N. Carlson ,&nbsp;P. Borkenau","doi":"10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2024.104491","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Some meta-perceivers have more insight than others into the impressions they make, but what is this meta-accuracy about? Do good meta-perceivers have insight into the unique impressions they make (dyadic meta-accuracy), or do they simply understand their reputation (generalized meta-accuracy)? In two studies, wemetaces in dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy among close others, as well as potential mechanisms and correlates. Results suggest that, among close others, there are good meta-perceivers of dyadic and generalized meta-accuracy. Good meta-perceivers of dyadic meta-accuracy form more differentiated meta-perceptions, while good meta-perceivers of generalized meta-accuracy make and think they make consistent impressions that align with their self-views. Thus, among close others – unlike in first impressions – there are good meta-perceivers who perspective-take.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48406,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Research in Personality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656624000394/pdfft?md5=24a3f4a11c8bfe0554c5cffe95e69c2a&pid=1-s2.0-S0092656624000394-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140618713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Research in Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1