首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Personality最新文献

英文 中文
Defining distress tolerance in a structural model of Big Five personality domains. 在大五人格领域的结构模型中定义痛苦容忍度。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12952
Maria Martin Lopez, Kristin Naragon-Gainey, Christopher C Conway

Objective: Distress tolerance (DT)-willingness to face internal discomforts-has a fuzzy boundary with neuroticism (low emotional stability), raising questions about its independent role in models of personality and mental health.

Method: We investigated DT's overlap with neuroticism and other Big Five factors in a structural model of personality and personality disorder features in samples of university students (N = 1025), emotional disorder patients (N = 225), and substance-use patients (N = 210).

Results: In exploratory factor analyses, we found that DT indicators clustered with neuroticism and were essentially unrelated to other Big Five domains. Big Five personality dimensions collectively explained approximately 40%-70% of variation in DT, across different samples and methods of quantifying shared variance.

Conclusions: We conclude that DT and neuroticism are near neighbors in empirical space and speculate that much of the observed correlation between DT and mental health outcomes in the literature may be carried by shared neuroticism variance. We suggest that clearer distinctions between the two constructs in empirical research could improve our understanding of DT's unique role in the development and treatment of psychopathology.

目的压力耐受性(DT)--面对内心不适的意愿--与神经质(情绪稳定性低)的界限模糊,这就对其在人格和心理健康模型中的独立作用提出了质疑:我们以大学生(1025 人)、情绪障碍患者(225 人)和药物滥用患者(210 人)为样本,研究了 DT 在人格和人格障碍特征结构模型中与神经质和其他大五因子的重叠:在探索性因子分析中,我们发现 DT 指标与神经质聚集在一起,与其他大五人格领域基本无关。在不同的样本和量化共同变异的方法中,大五人格维度共同解释了DT变异的约40%-70%:我们得出的结论是,DT 和神经质在经验空间中是近邻,并推测文献中观察到的 DT 和心理健康结果之间的相关性大部分可能是由共同的神经质变异带来的。我们建议,在实证研究中更明确地区分这两个概念,可以提高我们对 DT 在精神病理学的发展和治疗中的独特作用的理解。
{"title":"Defining distress tolerance in a structural model of Big Five personality domains.","authors":"Maria Martin Lopez, Kristin Naragon-Gainey, Christopher C Conway","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12952","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12952","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Distress tolerance (DT)-willingness to face internal discomforts-has a fuzzy boundary with neuroticism (low emotional stability), raising questions about its independent role in models of personality and mental health.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We investigated DT's overlap with neuroticism and other Big Five factors in a structural model of personality and personality disorder features in samples of university students (N = 1025), emotional disorder patients (N = 225), and substance-use patients (N = 210).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In exploratory factor analyses, we found that DT indicators clustered with neuroticism and were essentially unrelated to other Big Five domains. Big Five personality dimensions collectively explained approximately 40%-70% of variation in DT, across different samples and methods of quantifying shared variance.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>We conclude that DT and neuroticism are near neighbors in empirical space and speculate that much of the observed correlation between DT and mental health outcomes in the literature may be carried by shared neuroticism variance. We suggest that clearer distinctions between the two constructs in empirical research could improve our understanding of DT's unique role in the development and treatment of psychopathology.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201050","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who believes in a just world? A multilevel latent profile analysis of Justice Capital using the European Social Survey 谁相信世界是公正的?利用欧洲社会调查对正义资本进行多层次潜在特征分析。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12947
Jonathan Bartholomaeus

Objective

Justice Capital provides a theoretical framework for explaining individual differences in the belief in a just world (BJW). However, this framework has yet to receive empirical validation.

Method

Using Round 9 of the European Social Survey, a large (n = 43,209) multi-country (N = 29) sample, I conduct multilevel latent profile analysis and multilevel multinomial logistic regression to determine the latent profiles that emerge at a population level and map the demographic and experiential covariates of these profiles.

Results

Incorporating measures of general BJW, distributive and procedural justice, and the belief in equality of opportunity, I find three latent profiles: meritocrats, moderates, and egalitarians. Compared with egalitarians, meritocrats (strong just world believers) are more likely to be male; younger; have a higher income; have attained more years of education; to be politically conservative; and have no recent experience of discrimination or crime. Meritocrats were overrepresented in countries with a higher Human Development Index.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates the feasibility of Justice Capital for understanding individual variation in general BJW and related justice beliefs; discussion centers on anomalous findings and extension of this theoretical framework.

目的:正义资本(Justice Capital)提供了一个理论框架,用于解释个体在正义世界信念(BJW)方面的差异。然而,这一框架尚未得到实证验证:利用欧洲社会调查第 9 轮(一个大型(n = 43209)多国(N = 29)样本),我进行了多层次潜在特征分析和多层次多叉逻辑回归,以确定在人口层面出现的潜在特征,并绘制这些特征的人口和经验协变量图:通过对一般北京赛车、分配和程序公正以及机会均等信念的测量,我发现了三种潜在特征:任人唯贤者、温和主义者和平等主义者。与平等主义者相比,功利主义者(坚定的公正世界信仰者)更可能是男性;更年轻;收入更高;受教育年限更长;政治上保守;近期没有歧视或犯罪经历。在人类发展指数较高的国家,功利主义者的比例较高:本研究证明了 "正义资本 "在理解一般北京赛车和值技巧数学及相关正义信念的个体差异方面的可行性;讨论集中于异常发现和这一理论框架的扩展。
{"title":"Who believes in a just world? A multilevel latent profile analysis of Justice Capital using the European Social Survey","authors":"Jonathan Bartholomaeus","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12947","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12947","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Justice Capital provides a theoretical framework for explaining individual differences in the belief in a just world (BJW). However, this framework has yet to receive empirical validation.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Using Round 9 of the European Social Survey, a large (<i>n</i> = 43,209) multi-country (<i>N</i> = 29) sample, I conduct multilevel latent profile analysis and multilevel multinomial logistic regression to determine the latent profiles that emerge at a population level and map the demographic and experiential covariates of these profiles.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Incorporating measures of general BJW, distributive and procedural justice, and the belief in equality of opportunity, I find three latent profiles: meritocrats, moderates, and egalitarians. Compared with egalitarians, meritocrats (strong just world believers) are more likely to be male; younger; have a higher income; have attained more years of education; to be politically conservative; and have no recent experience of discrimination or crime. Meritocrats were overrepresented in countries with a higher Human Development Index.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusion</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study demonstrates the feasibility of Justice Capital for understanding individual variation in general BJW and related justice beliefs; discussion centers on anomalous findings and extension of this theoretical framework.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"449-462"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12947","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141162605","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Which emotion regulation strategy is efficient for whom? Reappraisal and suppression efficiency for adaptive and maladaptive personality profiles 哪种情绪调节策略对谁有效?适应型和适应不良型人格特征的重评和抑制效率。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12948
Elena Trentini, Elise Dan-Glauser

Objective

This study aimed to explore the efficiency of different emotion regulation strategies, specifically reappraisal and suppression, in relation to adaptive and maladaptive personality profiles.

Background

Personality conditions emotions and influences emotion regulation. Of the available regulation strategies, reappraisal (reinterpreting the situation) is described as an efficient strategy, whereas suppression (not displaying the experienced emotion) carries higher physiological and cognitive costs. Little is known, however, about the influence of personality on these efficiencies.

Method

We tested the personality structure of 102 participants (Meanage = 20.75, SDage = 2.15), based on the Five-Factor Model and the Maladaptive Personality Trait Model. Experience, expressivity, and physiological arousal were recorded during the viewing of emotionally charged positive and negative images while participants reappraised, suppressed, or viewed the images without regulating their emotions.

Results

We identified two clusters for adaptive personality (“Adaptive Resilient” and “Anti-resilient”) and two for maladaptive personality (“Maladaptive Resilient” and “Under-controlled”). The major finding was for emotional experience in maladaptive personalities, where reappraisal was efficient in the Maladaptive Resilient profile, while none of the strategies brought relief in the Under-controlled profile.

Conclusion

This study, which systematically contrasts personality and efficiency of emotion regulation strategies, is one of the first attempts to refine the understanding of how personality influences the emotional regulation process.

研究目的本研究旨在探讨不同情绪调节策略的效率,特别是重评和压抑,与适应性人格和适应不良人格的关系:背景:人格是情绪的条件并影响情绪调节。在现有的调节策略中,重新评价(重新解释情境)被认为是一种有效的策略,而压抑(不表现出所经历的情绪)则需要付出更高的生理和认知成本。然而,人们对人格对这些效率的影响知之甚少:我们根据五因素模型和适应不良人格特质模型,测试了 102 名参与者(平均年龄 = 20.75,最小年龄 = 2.15)的人格结构。在观看带有情绪色彩的正面和负面图像时,记录参与者的体验、表达能力和生理唤醒,同时重新评估、抑制或在不调节情绪的情况下观看图像:我们发现了两个适应性人格集群("适应性复原力 "和 "反复原力")和两个适应性不良人格集群("适应性不良复原力 "和 "控制不足")。主要发现是在适应不良型人格的情绪体验方面,重新评估在适应不良型复原人格中是有效的,而在控制不足型人格中,没有任何一种策略可以缓解情绪:这项研究系统地对比了人格和情绪调节策略的效率,是完善人格如何影响情绪调节过程的认识的首次尝试之一。
{"title":"Which emotion regulation strategy is efficient for whom? Reappraisal and suppression efficiency for adaptive and maladaptive personality profiles","authors":"Elena Trentini,&nbsp;Elise Dan-Glauser","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12948","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12948","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study aimed to explore the efficiency of different emotion regulation strategies, specifically reappraisal and suppression, in relation to adaptive and maladaptive personality profiles.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Personality conditions emotions and influences emotion regulation. Of the available regulation strategies, reappraisal (reinterpreting the situation) is described as an efficient strategy, whereas suppression (not displaying the experienced emotion) carries higher physiological and cognitive costs. Little is known, however, about the influence of personality on these efficiencies.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We tested the personality structure of 102 participants (Mean<sub>age</sub> = 20.75, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 2.15), based on the Five-Factor Model and the Maladaptive Personality Trait Model. Experience, expressivity, and physiological arousal were recorded during the viewing of emotionally charged positive and negative images while participants reappraised, suppressed, or viewed the images without regulating their emotions.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We identified two clusters for adaptive personality (“Adaptive Resilient” and “Anti-resilient”) and two for maladaptive personality (“Maladaptive Resilient” and “Under-controlled”). The major finding was for emotional experience in maladaptive personalities, where reappraisal was efficient in the Maladaptive Resilient profile, while none of the strategies brought relief in the Under-controlled profile.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusion</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study, which systematically contrasts personality and efficiency of emotion regulation strategies, is one of the first attempts to refine the understanding of how personality influences the emotional regulation process.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"463-488"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12948","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141155749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feelings as a currency of care: A role for agreeableness in emotional reactivity. 情感是关爱的货币:合意性在情绪反应中的作用。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12951
Michael D Robinson, Roberta L Irvin, Hamidreza Fereidouni, Robert J Klein

Objective and background: The personality trait of agreeableness is linked to a number of core tendencies (e.g., empathy, warmth) that operate in a feeling-based manner. Following considerations of this type, it is proposed that the motivations and characteristics of agreeable individuals, relative to disagreeable individuals, should render them more receptive to emotional events and more responsive to them for this reason.

Method: Potential links between agreeableness and emotional reactivity were assessed in two studies involving four samples (total N = 517) in which participants continuously rated their feeling states in response to a variety of affective images.

Results: Agreeableness did not predict the speed with which emotional reactions began, but agreeable individuals exhibited higher-magnitude peak intensities, regardless of whether stimuli were appetitive (pleasant) or aversive (unpleasant) in nature.

Conclusions: The findings provide novel insights into the personality trait of agreeableness, emotional reactivity phenomena, and the dynamic processes that link agreeableness to emotion.

目的和背景:合群的人格特质与一些以感情为基础的核心倾向(如同理心、温暖)有关。根据这种类型的考虑,有人提出,相对于不合群的人,合群的人的动机和特点应该使他们更容易接受情感事件,并因此对情感事件做出更多反应:方法:在两项涉及四个样本(总人数=517)的研究中,评估了宜人性与情绪反应性之间的潜在联系:结果表明:合意度并不能预测情绪反应开始的速度,但合意度高的人表现出更高的峰值强度,无论刺激物的性质是胃口性的(令人愉快的)还是厌恶性的(令人不愉快的):结论:这些研究结果为我们提供了关于 "合群 "这一人格特质、情绪反应现象以及将 "合群 "与情绪联系起来的动态过程的新见解。
{"title":"Feelings as a currency of care: A role for agreeableness in emotional reactivity.","authors":"Michael D Robinson, Roberta L Irvin, Hamidreza Fereidouni, Robert J Klein","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12951","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12951","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective and background: </strong>The personality trait of agreeableness is linked to a number of core tendencies (e.g., empathy, warmth) that operate in a feeling-based manner. Following considerations of this type, it is proposed that the motivations and characteristics of agreeable individuals, relative to disagreeable individuals, should render them more receptive to emotional events and more responsive to them for this reason.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Potential links between agreeableness and emotional reactivity were assessed in two studies involving four samples (total N = 517) in which participants continuously rated their feeling states in response to a variety of affective images.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Agreeableness did not predict the speed with which emotional reactions began, but agreeable individuals exhibited higher-magnitude peak intensities, regardless of whether stimuli were appetitive (pleasant) or aversive (unpleasant) in nature.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The findings provide novel insights into the personality trait of agreeableness, emotional reactivity phenomena, and the dynamic processes that link agreeableness to emotion.</p>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maladaptive personality traits and older adult relationship satisfaction: A co-twin control approach to understanding associations 适应不良的人格特质与老年人的关系满意度:用孪生兄弟控制法来理解两者之间的关联。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12949
Helen H. Yu, Colin D. Freilich, Sylia Wilson, Matt McGue, Glenn I. Roisman, Robert F. Krueger

Objective

Maladaptive personality traits have been implicated in romantic relationship dissatisfaction, but the etiology of those links and the degree to which they extend to other types of relationships are unclear. The purpose of this study was to examine associations between maladaptive personality traits and satisfaction in various relationships using a co-twin control design to identify potential environmental contributions.

Method

The sample consisted of 1340 older adult twin participants from the Minnesota Twin Registry (Mage = 70.3) that completed the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 Faceted Brief Form and Network of Relationships Inventory (Revised for Older Adults).

Results

Several maladaptive personality traits were phenotypically associated with relationship dissatisfaction, with detachment and negative affect having the largest effects. Further, within twin pair differences in detachment and negative affect were associated with greater relationship dissatisfaction, suggesting that observed associations were mediated partly by the unique environment, not solely the result of genetic and familial confounding. Both phenotypic and co-twin associations were strongest overall in the romantic partner relationship.

Conclusion

These findings support the notion that maladaptive personality traits are implicated in interpersonal dysfunction across multiple domains.

目的:适应不良型人格特质与恋爱关系中的不满情绪有关联,但这些关联的病因及其延伸至其他类型关系的程度尚不清楚。本研究的目的是采用双生子共同对照设计,研究适应不良人格特质与各种关系满意度之间的关联,以确定潜在的环境因素:样本由明尼苏达双胞胎登记处(Mage = 70.3)的 1340 名老年双胞胎参与者组成,他们完成了 DSM-5 面状简表人格量表和关系网络量表(针对老年人的修订版):几种适应不良的人格特质在表型上与人际关系不满有关,其中疏离和消极情绪的影响最大。此外,在一对双胞胎中,疏离和消极情绪的差异与更大的人际关系不满意度相关,这表明观察到的关联部分是由独特的环境介导的,而不仅仅是遗传和家族混杂的结果。在恋爱伴侣关系中,表型和同卵双生子的关联性总体上最强:这些研究结果支持这样一种观点,即适应不良的人格特质与多个领域的人际功能障碍有关。
{"title":"Maladaptive personality traits and older adult relationship satisfaction: A co-twin control approach to understanding associations","authors":"Helen H. Yu,&nbsp;Colin D. Freilich,&nbsp;Sylia Wilson,&nbsp;Matt McGue,&nbsp;Glenn I. Roisman,&nbsp;Robert F. Krueger","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12949","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12949","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Maladaptive personality traits have been implicated in romantic relationship dissatisfaction, but the etiology of those links and the degree to which they extend to other types of relationships are unclear. The purpose of this study was to examine associations between maladaptive personality traits and satisfaction in various relationships using a co-twin control design to identify potential environmental contributions.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The sample consisted of 1340 older adult twin participants from the Minnesota Twin Registry (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 70.3) that completed the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 Faceted Brief Form and Network of Relationships Inventory (Revised for Older Adults).</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Several maladaptive personality traits were phenotypically associated with relationship dissatisfaction, with detachment and negative affect having the largest effects. Further, within twin pair differences in detachment and negative affect were associated with greater relationship dissatisfaction, suggesting that observed associations were mediated partly by the unique environment, not solely the result of genetic and familial confounding. Both phenotypic and co-twin associations were strongest overall in the romantic partner relationship.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusion</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>These findings support the notion that maladaptive personality traits are implicated in interpersonal dysfunction across multiple domains.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"489-502"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12949","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141077121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High-dimensionality structure in English-language personality type-nouns 英语人格类型名词的高维度结构。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12940
Vinita Vader, Gerard Saucier

Objective

Past applications of the lexical approach to type-noun personality structures have yielded different results compared with those generated for adjectival personality structures, since then new methods have arisen for identifying robust higher-dimensionality structure in data. This research aims to identify an optimal taxonomy of English language type-nouns.

Method

Current study reanalyzed 372 type-nouns from a previous study emphasizing robustness across methodological variations (original vs. ipsatized data, oblique vs. orthogonal rotations, convergence between male and female target ratings) to determine a replicable but more comprehensive model of personality type-noun structure.

Results

A 13-factor original-data oblimin-rotated solution was determined to be the most robust model, except for a one-factor model that was far less comprehensive and informative; an original-data 32-factor oblimin-rotated solution was also fairly robust. Although each of the Big Five adjectival markers indicated a large correlation with one or more type-noun factors; nearly half of the 13 type-noun factors lacked such large correlations with the Big Five.

Conclusions

A high-dimensionality approach thus indicated that type-nouns capture substantial content beyond the Big Five. A comparison with the character-types described by an ancient philosopher (Theophrastus) signified that some granular type-noun dimensions may have stability across multiple millennia.

目的:过去将词法应用于类型名词人格结构时,与形容词人格结构相比,产生了不同的结果,此后出现了新的方法来识别数据中稳健的高维度结构。本研究旨在确定英语类型名词的最佳分类法:本研究重新分析了之前研究中的 372 个类型名词,强调了不同方法(原始数据与同位数据、斜向旋转与正交旋转、男性与女性目标评分的趋同性)之间的稳健性,以确定一个可复制但更全面的人格类型名词结构模型:结果:13 个因素的原始数据斜交旋转方案被认为是最稳健的模型,只有一个因素的模型在全面性和信息量方面要差得多;32 个因素的原始数据斜交旋转方案也相当稳健。虽然五大形容词标记中的每一个都与一个或多个类型名词因子有很大的相关性,但在 13 个类型名词因子中,有近一半与五大形容词标记没有很大的相关性:因此,高维度方法表明,类型名词捕捉到了五大因素之外的大量内容。与古代哲学家(Theophrastus)所描述的性格类型进行比较后发现,某些细化的类型名词维度可能具有跨越千年的稳定性。
{"title":"High-dimensionality structure in English-language personality type-nouns","authors":"Vinita Vader,&nbsp;Gerard Saucier","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12940","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12940","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Past applications of the lexical approach to type-noun personality structures have yielded different results compared with those generated for adjectival personality structures, since then new methods have arisen for identifying robust higher-dimensionality structure in data. This research aims to identify an optimal taxonomy of English language type-nouns.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Current study reanalyzed 372 type-nouns from a previous study emphasizing robustness across methodological variations (original vs. ipsatized data, oblique vs. orthogonal rotations, convergence between male and female target ratings) to determine a replicable but more comprehensive model of personality type-noun structure.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>A 13-factor original-data oblimin-rotated solution was determined to be the most robust model, except for a one-factor model that was far less comprehensive and informative; an original-data 32-factor oblimin-rotated solution was also fairly robust. Although each of the Big Five adjectival markers indicated a large correlation with one or more type-noun factors; nearly half of the 13 type-noun factors lacked such large correlations with the Big Five.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>A high-dimensionality approach thus indicated that type-nouns capture substantial content beyond the Big Five. A comparison with the character-types described by an ancient philosopher (Theophrastus) signified that some granular type-noun dimensions may have stability across multiple millennia.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"425-448"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960203","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating solitude as a tool for downregulation of daily arousal using ecological momentary assessments 利用生态学瞬间评估,研究独处作为下调日常唤醒的工具。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12939
Thuy-vy T. Nguyen, Delali Konu, Samuel Forbes

Objective

This research explored arousal levels as a motivating factor for solitude-seeking. We hypothesized that solitude becomes more desirable when high-arousal emotions were heightened and individual differences in extraversion and neuroticism would moderate this pattern.

Method

We tracked individuals' hourly experiences throughout a day. We assessed their high-arousal positive (e.g., excitement) and negative emotions (e.g., tension), whether they were alone or with others, and their preferred situation at the time of the signal. We gathered 4338 surveys from 362 participants, with 103 participants completing all hourly surveys.

Results

Preference for and incidence of solitude changed throughout the day. Contrary to our hypotheses, lagged analyses did not indicate high-arousal emotions predicting reports of being alone an hour later. However, individuals were more likely to express a preference for solitude while experiencing high-arousal negative emotions, and less so while experiencing positive emotions. Younger individuals display stronger preference for solitude during experiences of high-arousal negative emotions. Extraversion and neuroticism did not moderate these patterns.

Conclusions

The results highlight the distinctive appeal of solitude as a space for young adults to deal with negative emotions. We discussed how these findings are connected to existing literature and implications for future research.

目的:本研究探讨了作为寻求独处动机因素的唤醒水平。我们假设,当高唤醒情绪高涨时,独处会变得更受欢迎,而外向性和神经质的个体差异会缓和这种模式。我们评估了他们的高唤醒积极情绪(如兴奋)和消极情绪(如紧张),他们是独自一人还是与他人在一起,以及他们在发出信号时的首选情境。我们收集了 362 位参与者的 4338 份调查问卷,其中 103 位参与者完成了所有的小时调查。与我们的假设相反,滞后分析并没有表明高焦虑情绪会预测一小时后的独处报告。然而,当个体经历高焦虑负面情绪时,他们更倾向于表达独处的偏好,而在经历正面情绪时,这种偏好就会降低。年轻人在经历高焦虑负面情绪时更倾向于独处。结论:研究结果凸显了独处作为年轻人处理负面情绪的空间的独特魅力。我们讨论了这些发现与现有文献的联系以及对未来研究的影响。
{"title":"Investigating solitude as a tool for downregulation of daily arousal using ecological momentary assessments","authors":"Thuy-vy T. Nguyen,&nbsp;Delali Konu,&nbsp;Samuel Forbes","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12939","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12939","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This research explored arousal levels as a motivating factor for solitude-seeking. We hypothesized that solitude becomes more desirable when high-arousal emotions were heightened and individual differences in extraversion and neuroticism would moderate this pattern.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>We tracked individuals' hourly experiences throughout a day. We assessed their high-arousal positive (e.g., excitement) and negative emotions (e.g., tension), whether they were alone or with others, and their preferred situation at the time of the signal. We gathered 4338 surveys from 362 participants, with 103 participants completing all hourly surveys.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Preference for and incidence of solitude changed throughout the day. Contrary to our hypotheses, lagged analyses did not indicate high-arousal emotions predicting reports of being alone an hour later. However, individuals were more likely to express a preference for solitude while experiencing high-arousal negative emotions, and less so while experiencing positive emotions. Younger individuals display stronger preference for solitude during experiences of high-arousal negative emotions. Extraversion and neuroticism did not moderate these patterns.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The results highlight the distinctive appeal of solitude as a space for young adults to deal with negative emotions. We discussed how these findings are connected to existing literature and implications for future research.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 1","pages":"31-50"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/jopy.12939","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140963028","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relations among prosocial behavior, hedonic, and eudaimonic well-being in everyday life 日常生活中亲社会行为、享乐和幸福感之间的关系。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12937
Fulvio Gregori, Belén López-Pérez, Lucia Manfredi, Nancy Eisenberg, David Lundie, Shannon Lee, Maria Gerbino, Concetta Pastorelli, Antonio Zuffianò

Introduction

Existing research highlights the significance of prosocial behavior (voluntary, intentional behavior that results in benefits for another) to people's well-being. Yet, the extent to which this expected positive relation operates at the within-person level (e.g., is more prosocial behavior than usual related to a higher than usual level of well-being?) while taking into account stable interindividual differences, remains a research question that deserves further investigation. In this study, we aimed to explore the relations between prosocial behavior and hedonic (HWB; subjective assessment of life satisfaction and happiness) and eudaimonic (EWB; actualization of human potential in alignment with personal goals, including concepts like meaning in life and closeness to others) well-being in daily life.

Method

Using ecological momentary assessment for 4 weeks, data were collected from two British samples, comprising 82 adolescents and 166 adults.

Results

Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling revealed a positive relations between prosocial behavior and HWB/EWB at both between and within-person levels across the samples.

Conclusion

In summary, these findings further support the positive link between prosocial behavior and well-being in everyday life. Notably, this association was consistent across different age groups (adolescent and adults) at both between and within-person levels.

导言:现有研究强调了亲社会行为(为他人带来利益的自愿、有意行为)对人们幸福感的重要意义。然而,在考虑到稳定的个体间差异的同时,这种预期的积极关系在多大程度上是在个人内部层面上发挥作用的(例如,比通常情况下更多的亲社会行为是否与比通常情况下更高的幸福感水平相关?在这项研究中,我们旨在探讨日常生活中亲社会行为与享乐型(HWB;对生活满意度和幸福感的主观评估)和幸福型(EWB;根据个人目标实现人类潜能,包括生活意义和与他人亲近等概念)幸福感之间的关系:方法:采用生态学瞬间评估法,从两个英国样本(包括 82 名青少年和 166 名成年人)中收集了为期 4 周的数据:结果:动态结构方程模型显示,亲社会行为与 HWB/EWB 之间在样本间和样本内均存在正相关关系:总之,这些研究结果进一步证实了亲社会行为与日常生活幸福感之间的积极联系。值得注意的是,这种联系在不同年龄组(青少年和成年人)的人与人之间和人与人之间的层面上都是一致的。
{"title":"The relations among prosocial behavior, hedonic, and eudaimonic well-being in everyday life","authors":"Fulvio Gregori,&nbsp;Belén López-Pérez,&nbsp;Lucia Manfredi,&nbsp;Nancy Eisenberg,&nbsp;David Lundie,&nbsp;Shannon Lee,&nbsp;Maria Gerbino,&nbsp;Concetta Pastorelli,&nbsp;Antonio Zuffianò","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12937","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12937","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Introduction</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Existing research highlights the significance of prosocial behavior (voluntary, intentional behavior that results in benefits for another) to people's well-being. Yet, the extent to which this expected positive relation operates at the within-person level (e.g., is more prosocial behavior than usual related to a higher than usual level of well-being?) while taking into account stable interindividual differences, remains a research question that deserves further investigation. In this study, we aimed to explore the relations between prosocial behavior and hedonic (HWB; subjective assessment of life satisfaction and happiness) and eudaimonic (EWB; actualization of human potential in alignment with personal goals, including concepts like meaning in life and closeness to others) well-being in daily life.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Using ecological momentary assessment for 4 weeks, data were collected from two British samples, comprising 82 adolescents and 166 adults.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Dynamic Structural Equation Modeling revealed a positive relations between prosocial behavior and HWB/EWB at both between and within-person levels across the samples.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusion</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>In summary, these findings further support the positive link between prosocial behavior and well-being in everyday life. Notably, this association was consistent across different age groups (adolescent and adults) at both between and within-person levels.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"412-424"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140945235","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Alone but flowing: The effects of autotelic personality and extraversion on solitary flow 独来独往自律型人格和外向性对独处流动的影响。
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12938
Dwight C. K. Tse, Ayodele Joseph, Kate Sweeny

Objective/Background

Flow, a psychological state of intense engagement in and enjoyment of an activity, can arise during both solitary and socially interactive experiences. In the literature, whereas people high in extraversion have difficulty achieving flow in solitude, those with an autotelic personality—a combination of traits that make people prone to flow—readily experience flow in both solitary and interactive conditions. In this pre-registered experiment, we investigated whether autotelic personality mitigates the negative association between solitary flow and extraversion.

Method

Participants and their romantic partners (final N = 368) played the game Perfection™ in three conditions (order was counterbalanced): alone (solitary condition), in the presence of their partner without interaction (mere-presence condition), and collaboratively (interactive condition).

Results

There were independent, positive main effects of extraversion and autotelic personality on flow experience in mere-presence and interactive conditions. However, the positive effect of extraversion on solitary flow was only significant among participants with high (vs. low) autotelic personality. In all conditions, flow experience was associated with greater low-arousal positive affect and lesser high-arousal negative affect.

Conclusions

The findings shed light on the role of personality in promoting solitary flow experiences, and particularly how traits might interact to determine optimal and non-optimal conditions for achieving flow.

目的/背景:流动是一种强烈投入并享受某种活动的心理状态,既可在独处时产生,也可在社交互动时产生。在文献中,外向性高的人在独处时很难获得流动感,而那些具有自律型人格的人--一种使人容易产生流动感的特质组合--在独处和互动的条件下都很容易体验到流动感。在这项预先登记的实验中,我们研究了自恋型人格是否能减轻独处流动与外向性之间的负相关:方法:参与者和他们的恋爱伴侣(最终人数= 368)在三种条件下玩《完美™》游戏(顺序是平衡的):独处(独处条件)、伴侣在场但无互动(仅在场条件)和合作(互动条件):结果:在单纯在场和互动条件下,外向型人格和自律型人格对流动体验有独立的正向主效应。然而,外向性对独处流动的积极影响仅在高自恋型人格(与低自恋型人格)的参与者中显著。在所有条件下,流动体验都与较高的低唤醒积极情感和较低的高唤醒消极情感相关:研究结果揭示了人格在促进独处流动体验中的作用,特别是人格特质如何相互作用,以确定实现流动的最佳和非最佳条件。
{"title":"Alone but flowing: The effects of autotelic personality and extraversion on solitary flow","authors":"Dwight C. K. Tse,&nbsp;Ayodele Joseph,&nbsp;Kate Sweeny","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12938","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12938","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective/Background</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Flow, a psychological state of intense engagement in and enjoyment of an activity, can arise during both solitary and socially interactive experiences. In the literature, whereas people high in extraversion have difficulty achieving flow in solitude, those with an autotelic personality—a combination of traits that make people prone to flow—readily experience flow in both solitary and interactive conditions. In this pre-registered experiment, we investigated whether autotelic personality mitigates the negative association between solitary flow and extraversion.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Participants and their romantic partners (final <i>N</i> = 368) played the game Perfection™ in three conditions (order was counterbalanced): alone (<i>solitary condition</i>), in the presence of their partner without interaction (<i>mere-presence condition</i>), and collaboratively (<i>interactive condition</i>).</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>There were independent, positive main effects of extraversion and autotelic personality on flow experience in mere-presence and interactive conditions. However, the positive effect of extraversion on solitary flow was only significant among participants with high (vs. low) autotelic personality. In all conditions, flow experience was associated with greater low-arousal positive affect and lesser high-arousal negative affect.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The findings shed light on the role of personality in promoting solitary flow experiences, and particularly how traits might interact to determine optimal and non-optimal conditions for achieving flow.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 1","pages":"67-80"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11705519/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899735","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Notes from the Underground: Seeking the top personality correlates of self-referencing 地下笔记寻找自我参照的顶级人格相关因素
IF 5 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1111/jopy.12936
Nicholas S. Holtzman, Jeffrey J. Klibert, A. Brianna Dixon, Hannah L. Dorough, M. Brent Donnellan

Objective

Self-focused language use has been frequently assumed to reflect narcissism; however, research indicates that the association between first-person singular pronouns (i.e., “I-talk”) and grandiose narcissism is negligible.

Method

To extend this literature, we progressively identify vulnerable narcissism and rumination as positive correlates of I-talk in five studies (valid Ns = 211, 475, 1253, 289, 1113).

Results

The first study revealed positive correlates of I-talk suggestive of vulnerable narcissism. The second study showed more directly that vulnerable narcissism was a positive correlate but that this association was attributable to shared variance with neuroticism. The third study, a preregistered effort, replicated and extended the results of the second study. The fourth and fifth studies focused on rumination in a preregistered manner.

Conclusions

All the studies point to a clear distinction: While grandiose narcissism is negligibly related to I-talk, vulnerable narcissism is positively related to I-talk; moreover, rumination is a robust predictor of I-talk. A research synthesis revealed the following constructs significantly capture I-talk: depression (r = 0.10), neuroticism (r = 0.15), rumination (r = 0.14), and vulnerable narcissism (r = 0.12). The association between I-talk and neuroticism was partially mediated by rumination, providing a testable candidate mechanism for neuroticism interventions.

目标以自我为中心的语言使用经常被认为反映了自恋;然而,研究表明,第一人称单数代词(即 "我说")与自恋之间的关联微乎其微、方法为了扩展这些文献,我们在五项研究(有效样本数 = 211、475、1253、289、1113)中逐步将脆弱自恋和反刍确定为 "我说 "的正相关因素。结果第一项研究显示,"我说 "的正相关因素暗示了脆弱自恋。第二项研究更直接地表明,脆弱自恋是一种正相关,但这种关联可归因于神经质的共同变异。第三项研究是一项预先登记的工作,它重复并扩展了第二项研究的结果。第四项和第五项研究以预先登记的方式关注反刍:所有研究都指出了一个明显的区别:傲慢自恋与 "我说 "的关系微乎其微,而脆弱自恋与 "我说 "呈正相关;此外,反刍是 "我说 "的一个强有力的预测因素。研究综述显示,以下结构显著捕捉了我谈现象:抑郁(r = 0.10)、神经质(r = 0.15)、反刍(r = 0.14)和脆弱自恋(r = 0.12)。I-talk与神经质之间的关联部分是由反刍中介的,这为神经质干预提供了一个可测试的候选机制。
{"title":"Notes from the Underground: Seeking the top personality correlates of self-referencing","authors":"Nicholas S. Holtzman,&nbsp;Jeffrey J. Klibert,&nbsp;A. Brianna Dixon,&nbsp;Hannah L. Dorough,&nbsp;M. Brent Donnellan","doi":"10.1111/jopy.12936","DOIUrl":"10.1111/jopy.12936","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Objective</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Self-focused language use has been frequently assumed to reflect narcissism; however, research indicates that the association between first-person singular pronouns (i.e., “I-talk”) and grandiose narcissism is negligible.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Method</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>To extend this literature, we progressively identify vulnerable narcissism and rumination as positive correlates of I-talk in five studies (valid <i>N</i>s = 211, 475, 1253, 289, 1113).</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Results</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The first study revealed positive correlates of I-talk suggestive of vulnerable narcissism. The second study showed more directly that vulnerable narcissism was a positive correlate but that this association was attributable to shared variance with neuroticism. The third study, a preregistered effort, replicated and extended the results of the second study. The fourth and fifth studies focused on rumination in a preregistered manner.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Conclusions</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <p>All the studies point to a clear distinction: While grandiose narcissism is negligibly related to I-talk, vulnerable narcissism is positively related to I-talk; moreover, rumination is a robust predictor of I-talk. A research synthesis revealed the following constructs significantly capture I-talk: depression (<i>r</i> = 0.10), neuroticism (<i>r</i> = 0.15), rumination (<i>r</i> = 0.14), and vulnerable narcissism (<i>r</i> = 0.12). The association between I-talk and neuroticism was partially mediated by rumination, providing a testable candidate mechanism for neuroticism interventions.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48421,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Personality","volume":"93 2","pages":"394-411"},"PeriodicalIF":5.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140640061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Personality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1