首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
If not me then we: Goal tradeoffs in decision-making for the self, ingroup, and outgroup 如果不是我,那就是我们:自我、内群和外群决策中的目标权衡
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104625
Suraiya Allidina , William A. Cunningham

Navigating the social world requires individuals to balance multiple goals, including the drives to improve one's own outcomes, aid ingroup members, and help or hurt outgroup members. While self-interest and intergroup bias are both well-established motivational phenomena, less is known about how these goals may interact. Here we examine the nature of goal tradeoffs in intergroup decision-making using a novel task in which participants simultaneously make monetary decisions for themselves, an arbitrary ingroup, and the corresponding outgroup. Across four behavioural studies and one eye-tracking study (total N = 704), we find that goals in intergroup contexts are pursued sequentially rather than concurrently, with non-linear upweighting of group-related goals when self-related goals cannot be pursued. Further, we find evidence for stronger self-ingroup than self-outgroup tradeoffs, which manifest in both altered attention to information and altered use of the attended information in decision-making. The results shed light on the cognitive structuring of interrelated goals in intergroup decision-making, furthering our understanding of when and how both intergroup biases and prosocial behaviour may emerge.

在社会世界中游刃有余需要个人平衡多种目标,包括改善自身结果、帮助内群体成员以及帮助或伤害外群体成员。虽然自我利益和群体间偏差都是公认的动机现象,但人们对这些目标如何相互作用却知之甚少。在这里,我们通过一项新颖的任务,让参与者同时为自己、一个任意的内群体和相应的外群体做出金钱决策,来研究群体间决策中目标权衡的本质。通过四项行为研究和一项眼动追踪研究(总人数 = 704),我们发现在群体间情境中,目标是按顺序而非同时进行的,当无法实现自我相关目标时,群体相关目标的权重会非线性上升。此外,我们还发现了自我群体权衡强于自我群体权衡的证据,这种权衡表现为对信息的关注发生了变化,以及在决策中对所关注信息的使用发生了变化。这些结果揭示了群体间决策中相互关联目标的认知结构,进一步加深了我们对群体间偏见和亲社会行为何时以及如何出现的理解。
{"title":"If not me then we: Goal tradeoffs in decision-making for the self, ingroup, and outgroup","authors":"Suraiya Allidina ,&nbsp;William A. Cunningham","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104625","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104625","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Navigating the social world requires individuals to balance multiple goals, including the drives to improve one's own outcomes, aid ingroup members, and help or hurt outgroup members. While self-interest and intergroup bias are both well-established motivational phenomena, less is known about how these goals may interact. Here we examine the nature of goal tradeoffs in intergroup decision-making using a novel task in which participants simultaneously make monetary decisions for themselves, an arbitrary ingroup, and the corresponding outgroup. Across four behavioural studies and one eye-tracking study (total <em>N</em> = 704), we find that goals in intergroup contexts are pursued sequentially rather than concurrently, with non-linear upweighting of group-related goals when self-related goals cannot be pursued. Further, we find evidence for stronger self-ingroup than self-outgroup tradeoffs, which manifest in both altered attention to information and altered use of the attended information in decision-making. The results shed light on the cognitive structuring of interrelated goals in intergroup decision-making, furthering our understanding of when and how both intergroup biases and prosocial behaviour may emerge.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104625"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000374/pdfft?md5=09817b1f6630e64fef966104e0fdfb44&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000374-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140893551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social identity threat attenuates own-race bias in face recognition under the “Asian-Caucasian” context 在 "亚洲人-高加索人 "语境下,社会身份威胁可减轻人脸识别中的种族偏见
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104624
Yinxiu Gong, Xinyi Zhao, Qian Ma, Guomei Zhou

People typically remember faces of their own race better than the faces of other races, which is known as the own-race bias. Social-cognitive theories on the own-race bias debate if motivation plays a role in racial face recognition under the “Asian-Caucasian” context. We investigated the effect of social-identity-threat based motivation on own-race bias. In Experiment 1, Chinese participants were primed with a negative image of own-race by associating Chinese (i.e., own-race) faces with negative personality traits. Results showed that such social identity threat marginally affect the own-race bias in face recognition sensitivity (Experiment 1a), and it did not influence participants' implicit positive attitudes toward their ethnicity (Experiment 1b). In Experiments 2, 3 and 4, the social identity threat was manipulated by reading an article before the face recognition task. Experiment 2 found that the social identity threat significantly affected the own-race bias in face recognition false alarm. Although we did not find the modulation effect of group on ORB with ANOVA in Experiments 3 and 4, we observed the significant indirect effect of group on ORB through the mediator of generalized threat. Together, the current study provides evidence of social motivation in the own-race bias in the “Asian-Caucasian” setting.

人们对自己种族面孔的记忆通常好于对其他种族面孔的记忆,这就是所谓的 "种族偏见"。关于自身种族偏见的社会认知理论争论的焦点是,在 "亚洲-高加索 "背景下,动机是否在种族面孔识别中发挥作用。我们研究了基于社会身份威胁的动机对自身种族偏见的影响。在实验 1 中,通过将中国人(即自己的种族)面孔与负面人格特质联系起来,中国被试被引出了自己种族的负面形象。结果表明,这种社会身份威胁对人脸识别敏感度中的本族偏差有轻微影响(实验 1a),并且不影响参与者对本族的内隐积极态度(实验 1b)。在实验 2、3 和 4 中,通过在人脸识别任务前阅读一篇文章来操纵社会身份威胁。实验 2 发现,社会身份威胁显著影响了人脸识别误报中的自身种族偏差。虽然在实验 3 和 4 中,我们通过方差分析没有发现群体对错误警报的调节效应,但我们通过泛化威胁这一中介观察到了群体对错误警报的显著间接效应。总之,本研究为 "亚裔-高加索人 "背景下的自身种族偏差提供了社会动机的证据。
{"title":"Social identity threat attenuates own-race bias in face recognition under the “Asian-Caucasian” context","authors":"Yinxiu Gong,&nbsp;Xinyi Zhao,&nbsp;Qian Ma,&nbsp;Guomei Zhou","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104624","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104624","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People typically remember faces of their own race better than the faces of other races, which is known as the own-race bias. Social-cognitive theories on the own-race bias debate if motivation plays a role in racial face recognition under the “Asian-Caucasian” context. We investigated the effect of social-identity-threat based motivation on own-race bias. In Experiment 1, Chinese participants were primed with a negative image of own-race by associating Chinese (i.e., own-race) faces with negative personality traits. Results showed that such social identity threat marginally affect the own-race bias in face recognition sensitivity (Experiment 1a), and it did not influence participants' implicit positive attitudes toward their ethnicity (Experiment 1b). In Experiments 2, 3 and 4, the social identity threat was manipulated by reading an article before the face recognition task. Experiment 2 found that the social identity threat significantly affected the own-race bias in face recognition false alarm. Although we did not find the modulation effect of group on ORB with ANOVA in Experiments 3 and 4, we observed the significant indirect effect of group on ORB through the mediator of generalized threat. Together, the current study provides evidence of social motivation in the own-race bias in the “Asian-Caucasian” setting.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104624"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140823880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In the pursuit of happiness: Attaining a greater number of high-status positions increases well-being but only in select groups 在追求幸福的过程中:获得更多的高地位职位会增加幸福感,但仅限于特定群体
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104622
John Angus D. Hildreth

In the pursuit of happiness, how does an individual's standing in each of their groups affect their well-being? In ten pre-registered studies of 3554 participants, I found that attaining a greater number of high-status positions increased well-being but only in select groups. In surveys of workers (Studies 1, S1 and S2) and student athletes (Study S3), well-being was significantly positively related to the number and proportion of high-status positions a person held in their important groups, i.e., those groups central to their identity, but was not related to the status they held in their unimportant groups, regardless of how status was measured. Holding high-status in important groups increased well-being because such positions bolstered individuals' self-esteem and increased their sense of acceptance in those groups but not because such positions enhanced their sense of power. Four experiments (Studies 2, S5, S6, and S7) utilizing random assignment and a year-long longitudinal study (Study 3) established the causal relationship between well-being and high-status across groups as well as the moderating role of the groups' importance. A field study of graduate students (Study S4) utilizing a round-robin design confirmed that well-being was positively related to graduate students' self-reported status as well as the status ratings they received from their peers in an important group but not in an unimportant group. Therefore, in the pursuit of happiness, individuals would be wise to focus their energy on attaining and maintaining high-status only in those important groups that are central to their identity.

在追求幸福的过程中,个人在各个群体中的地位如何影响他们的幸福感?在对 3554 名参与者进行的 10 项预先登记的研究中,我发现获得更多的高地位职位会增加幸福感,但仅限于特定群体。在对工人(研究 1、S1 和 S2)和学生运动员(研究 S3)的调查中,幸福感与一个人在其重要群体(即对其身份认同至关重要的群体)中拥有的高地位职位的数量和比例呈显著正相关,但与他们在不重要群体中的地位无关,无论地位是如何衡量的。在重要群体中拥有较高的地位会增加幸福感,这是因为这些地位增强了个人的自尊心,提高了他们在这些群体中的被接纳感,而不是因为这些地位增强了他们的权力感。四项采用随机分配的实验(研究 2、研究 5、研究 6 和研究 7)和一项为期一年的纵向研究(研究 3)确定了幸福感与在不同群体中的高地位之间的因果关系,以及群体重要性的调节作用。一项采用循环设计对研究生进行的实地研究(研究 S4)证实,幸福感与研究生自我报告的地位以及他们在重要群体中从同伴那里获得的地位评价呈正相关,而在不重要群体中则不然。因此,在追求幸福的过程中,个人最好只在那些对其身份认同至关重要的重要群体中集中精力获得并保持较高的地位。
{"title":"In the pursuit of happiness: Attaining a greater number of high-status positions increases well-being but only in select groups","authors":"John Angus D. Hildreth","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104622","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104622","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the pursuit of happiness, how does an individual's standing in each of their groups affect their well-being? In ten pre-registered studies of 3554 participants, I found that attaining a greater number of high-status positions increased well-being but only in select groups. In surveys of workers (Studies 1, S1 and S2) and student athletes (Study S3), well-being was significantly positively related to the number and proportion of high-status positions a person held in their <em>important</em> groups, i.e., those groups central to their identity, but was not related to the status they held in their unimportant groups, regardless of how status was measured. Holding high-status in important groups increased well-being because such positions bolstered individuals' self-esteem and increased their sense of acceptance in those groups but not because such positions enhanced their sense of power. Four experiments (Studies 2, S5, S6, and S7) utilizing random assignment and a year-long longitudinal study (Study 3) established the causal relationship between well-being and high-status across groups as well as the moderating role of the groups' importance. A field study of graduate students (Study S4) utilizing a round-robin design confirmed that well-being was positively related to graduate students' self-reported status as well as the status ratings they received from their peers in an important group but not in an unimportant group. Therefore, in the pursuit of happiness, individuals would be wise to focus their energy on attaining and maintaining high-status only in those important groups that are central to their identity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104622"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140823881","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of financial stress on inhibitory control and economic decisions 经济压力对抑制控制和经济决策的影响
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104621
Bradley T. Hughes , Rita M. Ludwig , Kelly E. Robles , Elliot T. Berkman

Financial scarcity, both real and imagined, is associated with impaired executive functions and present-focused economic decisions. What is the mechanism that connects the lack of financial resources to these cognitive and behavioral effects? The present work will test the hypothesis that the experience of financial stress contributes to these deficits by reducing executive functions related to self-control and causing present-focused, real-world economic decisions. In a preliminary experiment (N = 215), we found support for the hypothesis that financial stress (as compared to social stress) causes a reduction in inhibitory control performance. In the registered study, we recruited participants (N = 1014) from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds and assessed inhibitory control before and after a financial stress manipulation, and economic preferences. The results did not support the hypothesis that momentary financial stress reduces inhibitory control or alters time preference. However, chronic financial stress was associated with reduced inhibitory control and VWM, and real-world economic decisions. Several interactions between SES and the effect of conditions highlight the relevance of a person's SES in the association between affective experiences and cognitive and behavioral responses. We discuss the implications of this work for future study of the association between SES, executive function, and economic decisions.

现实和想象中的资金匮乏都与执行功能受损和注重当前的经济决策有关。是什么机制将财务资源匮乏与这些认知和行为影响联系在一起?本研究将验证一个假设,即经济压力会降低与自我控制有关的执行功能,并导致以当前为中心的现实世界经济决策,从而导致这些缺陷。在一项初步实验(N = 215)中,我们发现财务压力(与社会压力相比)会导致抑制控制性能下降的假设得到了支持。在注册研究中,我们招募了来自不同社会经济背景的参与者(N = 1014),评估了经济压力操纵前后的抑制控制能力和经济偏好。结果并不支持瞬间经济压力会降低抑制控制能力或改变时间偏好的假设。然而,长期的经济压力与抑制控制能力和VWM的降低以及现实世界的经济决策有关。社会经济地位与条件影响之间的几种相互作用凸显了一个人的社会经济地位在情感体验与认知和行为反应之间的关联性。我们将讨论这项工作对未来研究社会经济地位、执行功能和经济决策之间关系的影响。
{"title":"The effect of financial stress on inhibitory control and economic decisions","authors":"Bradley T. Hughes ,&nbsp;Rita M. Ludwig ,&nbsp;Kelly E. Robles ,&nbsp;Elliot T. Berkman","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104621","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104621","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Financial scarcity, both real and imagined, is associated with impaired executive functions and present-focused economic decisions. What is the mechanism that connects the lack of financial resources to these cognitive and behavioral effects? The present work will test the hypothesis that the experience of financial stress contributes to these deficits by reducing executive functions related to self-control and causing present-focused, real-world economic decisions. In a preliminary experiment (<em>N</em> = 215), we found support for the hypothesis that financial stress (as compared to social stress) causes a reduction in inhibitory control performance. In the registered study, we recruited participants (<em>N</em> = 1014) from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds and assessed inhibitory control before and after a financial stress manipulation, and economic preferences. The results did not support the hypothesis that momentary financial stress reduces inhibitory control or alters time preference. However, chronic financial stress was associated with reduced inhibitory control and VWM, and real-world economic decisions. Several interactions between SES and the effect of conditions highlight the relevance of a person's SES in the association between affective experiences and cognitive and behavioral responses. We discuss the implications of this work for future study of the association between SES, executive function, and economic decisions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104621"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000337/pdfft?md5=d75e1d310cf0120a029922226e280950&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000337-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140818238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The spillover effect of mimicry: Being mimicked by one person increases prosocial behavior toward another person 模仿的溢出效应:被一个人模仿会增加对另一个人的亲社会行为
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104620
Paweł Muniak , Oliver Genschow , Dariusz Dolinski , Tomasz Grzyb , Wojciech Kulesza

People have the automatic tendency to mimic their interaction partners. Mimicry theories propose that such mimicking behavior is beneficial for the mimicker as mimicked persons tend to like, trust and help the mimicker more. Yet an open question remains as to whether prosocial effects translate to parties other than the mimicker. To test for the presence of such a spillover effect, we ran two field experiments (total N = 460). In all experiments, participants interacted with an experimenter. The experimenter either verbally mimicked the participants or behaved naturally. Afterwards, either the experimenter or another person asked participants to donate to a charity. Across all experiments, our results indicate that irrespective of whether a donation request is made by the mimicker or another person, mimicry increases the likelihood to donate to a charity, but not the amount that participants are willing to donate. Bayesian analyses suggest that this effect is less strongly pronounced than assumed by previous research and theories.

人们会自动模仿他们的互动伙伴。模仿理论认为,这种模仿行为对模仿者有利,因为被模仿者往往更喜欢、信任和帮助模仿者。然而,亲社会效应是否会转化到模仿者以外的其他人身上,这仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。为了测试是否存在这种溢出效应,我们进行了两次实地实验(总人数 = 460)。在所有实验中,参与者都与一名实验者进行互动。实验者要么口头模仿参与者,要么表现自然。之后,实验者或其他人要求参与者向慈善机构捐款。在所有实验中,我们的结果表明,不管是模仿者还是其他人提出捐赠要求,模仿都会增加参与者向慈善机构捐赠的可能性,但不会增加参与者愿意捐赠的金额。贝叶斯分析表明,这种效应没有以往的研究和理论所假设的那么明显。
{"title":"The spillover effect of mimicry: Being mimicked by one person increases prosocial behavior toward another person","authors":"Paweł Muniak ,&nbsp;Oliver Genschow ,&nbsp;Dariusz Dolinski ,&nbsp;Tomasz Grzyb ,&nbsp;Wojciech Kulesza","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104620","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104620","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>People have the automatic tendency to mimic their interaction partners. Mimicry theories propose that such mimicking behavior is beneficial for the mimicker as mimicked persons tend to like, trust and help the mimicker more. Yet an open question remains as to whether prosocial effects translate to parties other than the mimicker. To test for the presence of such a spillover effect, we ran two field experiments (total <em>N</em> = 460). In all experiments, participants interacted with an experimenter. The experimenter either verbally mimicked the participants or behaved naturally. Afterwards, either the experimenter or another person asked participants to donate to a charity. Across all experiments, our results indicate that irrespective of whether a donation request is made by the mimicker or another person, mimicry increases the likelihood to donate to a charity, but not the amount that participants are willing to donate. Bayesian analyses suggest that this effect is less strongly pronounced than assumed by previous research and theories.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104620"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645867","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Religiosity predicts the delegation of decisions between moral and self-serving immoral outcomes 宗教信仰可预测道德结果和自私自利的不道德结果之间的决策授权
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605
Alexa Weiss , Matthias Forstmann

Studies support an association between religious belief and prosocial behavior. Such religious prosociality has been attributed to fear of supernatural punishment and enhanced concern for a prosocial reputation and self-image. Hence, religious individuals may be more prone to pursue their self-interest indirectly, thereby averting personal responsibility. We conducted 12 studies (Ntotal = 4468) to examine whether religiosity predicts delegation in incentivized deception, dictator, and die-roll cheating games and in realistic scenarios. Participants could choose between an immoral (e.g., lying) and a moral, prosocial (e.g., honest/fair) option or leave this decision to another individual (the agent) who equally benefited from the immoral option. Across all studies, religiosity positively predicted delegation, even though participants could directly implement prosocial outcomes. Employing experimental manipulations of participants' interests, we found that the predictive effect of religiosity on delegation only emerged when participants could expect to benefit from the agent's decision, but not when they were not affected by it or could be harmed by it. At the same time, religiosity predicted prosocial decisions among non-delegating participants. Moreover, delegating participants felt less bad and responsible about their decisions and victims' outcomes. Taken together, these findings suggest that delegation is strategically employed by individuals who would otherwise act prosocially to pursue selfish interests while avoiding responsibility and blame. They further support the notion of religious prosociality as a multi-faceted, context-dependent phenomenon.

研究支持宗教信仰与亲社会行为之间的联系。这种宗教亲社会性被归因于对超自然惩罚的恐惧,以及对亲社会声誉和自我形象的更多关注。因此,信教者可能更容易间接追求自身利益,从而逃避个人责任。我们进行了 12 项研究(总人数 = 4468 人),以考察宗教信仰是否会预测在激励欺骗、独裁者和掷骰子作弊游戏以及现实场景中的委托行为。参与者可以在不道德(如撒谎)和道德、亲社会(如诚实/公平)选项之间做出选择,也可以将这一决定权交给另一个人(代理人),后者同样可以从不道德选项中获益。在所有研究中,即使参与者可以直接执行亲社会结果,宗教信仰对委托也有积极的预测作用。通过实验操纵参与者的利益,我们发现,只有当参与者可以预期从代理人的决定中获益时,宗教信仰才会对委托产生预测效应,而当参与者不受代理人决定的影响或可能受到代理人决定的损害时,宗教信仰就不会产生预测效应。同时,宗教信仰对非委托参与者的亲社会决策也有预测作用。此外,授权参与者对自己的决定和受害者的结果感到不那么糟糕,也不那么有责任感。综上所述,这些研究结果表明,委托是个人为了追求私利、避免责任和指责而采取的亲社会行为的策略性手段。这些发现进一步支持了宗教亲社会性是一种多方面的、依赖于情境的现象这一观点。
{"title":"Religiosity predicts the delegation of decisions between moral and self-serving immoral outcomes","authors":"Alexa Weiss ,&nbsp;Matthias Forstmann","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104605","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Studies support an association between religious belief and prosocial behavior. Such <em>religious prosociality</em> has been attributed to fear of supernatural punishment and enhanced concern for a prosocial reputation and self-image. Hence, religious individuals may be more prone to pursue their self-interest indirectly, thereby averting personal responsibility. We conducted 12 studies (<em>N</em><sub>total</sub> = 4468) to examine whether religiosity predicts delegation in incentivized deception, dictator, and die-roll cheating games and in realistic scenarios. Participants could choose between an immoral (e.g., lying) and a moral, prosocial (e.g., honest/fair) option or leave this decision to another individual (the agent) who equally benefited from the immoral option. Across all studies, religiosity positively predicted delegation, even though participants could directly implement prosocial outcomes. Employing experimental manipulations of participants' interests, we found that the predictive effect of religiosity on delegation only emerged when participants could expect to benefit from the agent's decision, but not when they were not affected by it or could be harmed by it. At the same time, religiosity predicted prosocial decisions among non-delegating participants. Moreover, delegating participants felt less bad and responsible about their decisions and victims' outcomes. Taken together, these findings suggest that delegation is strategically employed by individuals who would otherwise act prosocially to pursue selfish interests while avoiding responsibility and blame. They further support the notion of religious prosociality as a multi-faceted, context-dependent phenomenon.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104605"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140778917","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) 不确定性、专业知识和说服力:Karmarkar 和 Tormala(2010 年)的复制与扩展
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619
Erik Løhre , Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar , Lewend Mayiwar , Thorvald Hærem

If you are trying to persuade someone, expressing your opinion with certainty intuitively seems like a good strategy to maximize your influence. However, Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) found that the effectiveness of this tactic depends on expertise. In three experiments, Karmarkar and Tormala found support for an incongruity hypothesis, whereby non-expert sources can gain interest and influence by expressing certainty, while expert sources can increase persuasion by expressing uncertainty. In this Registered Report, we conducted a high-powered (N = 1018) direct replication of Experiment 2 by Karmarkar and Tormala (2010). In a consumer behaviour context, the original study examined whether source expertise moderated the positive effect of source certainty on the persuasive impact of a restaurant recommendation. The present replication failed to find support for the incongruity hypothesis, ηp2 = 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]: expressing certainty had a positive but non-significant effect for non-experts, d = 0.10 [−0.10, 0.34], and a positive effect for experts, d = 0.28 [0.03, 0.52]. Instead, the results supported the competing confidence heuristic hypothesis that expressed certainty would have a positive effect on persuasion, irrespective of source expertise, d = 0.18 [0.01, 0.36]. Extending the original work, we (1) controlled for the reason given for (un)certainty, and (2) examined need for closure as a potential individual difference moderator. The results indicated robust support for the confidence heuristic d = 0.25, [0.12, 0.37], but neither reason for (un)certainty nor need for closure moderated the effect as hypothesized. All materials, data, and code are available on: https://osf.io/hbjyv/.

如果你想说服别人,凭直觉肯定地表达自己的观点似乎是最大化影响力的好策略。然而,Karmarkar 和 Tormala(2010 年)发现,这种策略的有效性取决于专业知识。在三个实验中,Karmarkar 和 Tormala 发现不协调假说得到了支持,即非专家消息来源可以通过表达确定性来获得兴趣和影响力,而专家消息来源则可以通过表达不确定性来增加说服力。在本注册报告中,我们对 Karmarkar 和 Tormala(2010 年)的实验 2 进行了高功率(N = 1018)直接复制。在消费者行为的背景下,最初的研究考察了信息来源的专业知识是否会调节信息来源的确定性对餐厅推荐的说服力的积极影响。目前的重复研究未能发现对不一致性假设的支持,ηp2 = 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]:表达确定性对非专家有积极但不显著的影响,d = 0.10 [-0.10, 0.34],对专家有积极影响,d = 0.28 [0.03, 0.52]。相反,研究结果支持竞争信心启发式假设,即无论信息来源的专业知识如何,所表达的确定性都会对说服产生积极影响,d = 0.18 [0.01, 0.36]。在原有研究的基础上,我们(1)控制了(不)确定性的原因,(2)研究了作为潜在个体差异调节因素的封闭性需求。结果表明,信心启发式得到了强有力的支持 d = 0.25, [0.12, 0.37],但(不)确定的原因和封闭性需求都没有像假设的那样调节效果。所有材料、数据和代码可在以下网站获取:https://osf.io/hbjyv/。
{"title":"Uncertainty, expertise, and persuasion: A replication and extension of Karmarkar and Tormala (2010)","authors":"Erik Løhre ,&nbsp;Subramanya Prasad Chandrashekar ,&nbsp;Lewend Mayiwar ,&nbsp;Thorvald Hærem","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104619","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>If you are trying to persuade someone, expressing your opinion with certainty intuitively seems like a good strategy to maximize your influence. However, Karmarkar and Tormala (2010) found that the effectiveness of this tactic depends on expertise. In three experiments, Karmarkar and Tormala found support for an incongruity hypothesis, whereby non-expert sources can gain interest and influence by expressing certainty, while expert sources can increase persuasion by expressing uncertainty. In this Registered Report, we conducted a high-powered (<em>N</em> = 1018) direct replication of Experiment 2 by Karmarkar and Tormala (2010). In a consumer behaviour context, the original study examined whether source expertise moderated the positive effect of source certainty on the persuasive impact of a restaurant recommendation. The present replication failed to find support for the incongruity hypothesis, <em>η</em><sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.00 [0.00, 0.02]: expressing certainty had a positive but non-significant effect for non-experts, <em>d</em> = 0.10 [−0.10, 0.34], and a positive effect for experts, <em>d</em> = 0.28 [0.03, 0.52]. Instead, the results supported the competing <em>confidence heuristic</em> hypothesis that expressed certainty would have a positive effect on persuasion, irrespective of source expertise, <em>d</em> = 0.18 [0.01, 0.36]. Extending the original work, we (1) controlled for the reason given for (un)certainty, and (2) examined need for closure as a potential individual difference moderator. The results indicated robust support for the confidence heuristic <em>d</em> = 0.25, [0.12, 0.37], but neither reason for (un)certainty nor need for closure moderated the effect as hypothesized. All materials, data, and code are available on: <span>https://osf.io/hbjyv/</span><svg><path></path></svg>.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104619"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000313/pdfft?md5=8234e9b0b3218c8ce87a65d11bab3ec6&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000313-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140618356","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the bounded generalized reciprocity model: Ingroup favoritism and concerns about negative evaluation 重新审视有界广义互惠模型:同类偏爱和对负面评价的担忧
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618
Yutaka Horita, Shun Hamada

The bounded generalized reciprocity (BGR) model, grounded in reputation management, predicts that the motivation underlying ingroup favoritism (favoring one's own group over other groups) is driven by avoiding a negative reputation within one's own group. This research conducted two economic games with minimal groups in which reputational concerns (partners' knowledge of participants' group membership) were manipulated. We aimed to verify the replicability of the experimental results in support of the BGR model. A study (N = 394) using a dictator game (in which participants unilaterally determined their partners' payoffs) indicated the following: (1) participants were more likely to behave cooperatively with ingroup partners than with outgroup partners, regardless of whether their partners knew the participants' group membership; and (2) individual differences in fear of negative evaluation by others were not associated with cooperation toward the ingroup. Similar results were found in another study (N = 429) using a prisoner's dilemma game (in which participants' payoffs were determined by their partners). However, while sharing knowledge about group membership facilitated cooperation with the outgroup in the dictator game, no such tendency was observed in the prisoner's dilemma game. These findings suggest that concerns about a bad reputation may not play a relatively important role in ingroup favoritism, and that generosity toward outgroup members is influenced by the presence or absence of interdependence (i.e., whether the partner's behavior affects the participant's payoffs). This research proposes a reconsideration of the motivations behind cooperation within or between groups.

以声誉管理为基础的有界广义互惠(BGR)模型预测,内群体偏爱(偏爱本群体而非其他群体)的动机是为了避免在本群体中获得负面声誉。本研究进行了两个最小群体的经济博弈,其中对声誉问题(合作伙伴对参与者群体成员身份的了解)进行了操纵。我们旨在验证实验结果的可复制性,以支持 BGR 模型。一项使用独裁者游戏(参与者单方面决定其伙伴的报酬)进行的研究(N = 394)显示了以下结果:(1) 无论参与者的伙伴是否知道参与者的群体成员身份,参与者与内群体伙伴合作的可能性都高于与外群体伙伴合作的可能性;(2) 害怕他人负面评价的个体差异与对内群体的合作无关。另一项使用囚徒困境游戏(参与者的报酬由其伙伴决定)进行的研究(N = 429)也发现了类似的结果。然而,在独裁者游戏中,分享关于群体成员的知识有利于与外群体合作,而在囚徒困境游戏中却没有观察到这种趋势。这些研究结果表明,对坏名声的担忧可能不会在内向群体偏袒中起到相对重要的作用,而对外向群体成员的慷慨则会受到是否存在相互依赖关系(即伙伴的行为是否会影响参与者的回报)的影响。这项研究建议重新考虑群体内部或群体之间合作的动机。
{"title":"Revisiting the bounded generalized reciprocity model: Ingroup favoritism and concerns about negative evaluation","authors":"Yutaka Horita,&nbsp;Shun Hamada","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104618","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The bounded generalized reciprocity (BGR) model, grounded in reputation management, predicts that the motivation underlying ingroup favoritism (favoring one's own group over other groups) is driven by avoiding a negative reputation within one's own group. This research conducted two economic games with minimal groups in which reputational concerns (partners' knowledge of participants' group membership) were manipulated. We aimed to verify the replicability of the experimental results in support of the BGR model. A study (<em>N</em> = 394) using a dictator game (in which participants unilaterally determined their partners' payoffs) indicated the following: (1) participants were more likely to behave cooperatively with ingroup partners than with outgroup partners, regardless of whether their partners knew the participants' group membership; and (2) individual differences in fear of negative evaluation by others were not associated with cooperation toward the ingroup. Similar results were found in another study (<em>N</em> = 429) using a prisoner's dilemma game (in which participants' payoffs were determined by their partners). However, while sharing knowledge about group membership facilitated cooperation with the outgroup in the dictator game, no such tendency was observed in the prisoner's dilemma game. These findings suggest that concerns about a bad reputation may not play a relatively important role in ingroup favoritism, and that generosity toward outgroup members is influenced by the presence or absence of interdependence (i.e., whether the partner's behavior affects the participant's payoffs). This research proposes a reconsideration of the motivations behind cooperation within or between groups.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104618"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140552449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rude or just blunt? Honor, dignity, and spontaneous trait inferences from potentially offensive behaviors 粗鲁还是直率?从潜在的冒犯行为中推断荣誉、尊严和自发特质
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617
Ceren Günsoy , Irmak Olcaysoy Okten , A. Demaske

To restore their reputation, people from honor cultures (e.g., U.S. South) are more likely than people from dignity cultures (e.g., U.S. North) to retaliate against conflict partners who insult them. If a conflict partner does not insult them, however, they are more polite than dignity culture individuals, so that they don't provoke the person unnecessarily. Previous research has not examined the implicit person perception phase in these interactions. In this research, we focused on spontaneous trait inferences (STIs) that people can make from others' potentially offensive behaviors. In four studies (n = 1126), we tested whether being from a U.S. honor or dignity culture or the endorsement of these values was associated with hostile and nonhostile STIs, and whether honor and dignity influenced the relationship between STIs and behavioral intentions. In Study 1 and 2, honor culture participants made both types of STIs, whereas dignity culture participants only made hostile STIs. Study 3 revealed a positive association between individual honor endorsement and nonhostile STIs. In Study 4, we replicated most of these results and also found a positive association between hostile STIs and confrontation intentions in the honor (but not dignity) group. These results suggest that people from honor (vs. dignity) cultures tend to make rather simultaneous inferences of hostile and nonhostile traits from potentially offensive behaviors and making spontaneous hostile inferences can be more consequential for them. This research highlights the complexity of culture's influence on interpersonal processes and can have implications for diverse social contexts.

为了恢复名誉,荣誉文化(如美国南方)的人比尊严文化(如美国北方)的人更有可能对侮辱他们的冲突伙伴进行报复。然而,如果冲突伙伴没有侮辱他们,他们会比尊严文化的人更有礼貌,以免不必要地激怒对方。以前的研究没有考察过这些互动中的隐性人际感知阶段。在这项研究中,我们重点研究了人们从他人的潜在冒犯行为中做出的自发特质推断(STI)。在四项研究(n = 1126)中,我们测试了来自美国荣誉或尊严文化或对这些价值观的认可是否与敌意和非敌意的 STI 相关,以及荣誉和尊严是否会影响 STI 与行为意向之间的关系。在研究 1 和研究 2 中,荣誉文化参与者做出了两种类型的性传播感染,而尊严文化参与者只做出了敌意性传播感染。研究 3 表明,个人荣誉认可与非敌意性传播感染之间存在正相关。在研究 4 中,我们重复了上述大部分结果,并发现在荣誉(而非尊严)组中,敌意性传播感染与对抗意向之间存在正相关。这些结果表明,来自荣誉(与尊严)文化的人倾向于同时从潜在的冒犯行为中推断出敌意和非敌意特征,而自发地做出敌意推断可能会对他们造成更大的影响。这项研究凸显了文化对人际交往过程影响的复杂性,并可能对不同的社会环境产生影响。
{"title":"Rude or just blunt? Honor, dignity, and spontaneous trait inferences from potentially offensive behaviors","authors":"Ceren Günsoy ,&nbsp;Irmak Olcaysoy Okten ,&nbsp;A. Demaske","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104617","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>To restore their reputation, people from honor cultures (e.g., U.S. South) are more likely than people from dignity cultures (e.g., U.S. North) to retaliate against conflict partners who insult them. If a conflict partner does not insult them, however, they are more polite than dignity culture individuals, so that they don't provoke the person unnecessarily. Previous research has not examined the implicit person perception phase in these interactions. In this research, we focused on spontaneous trait inferences (STIs) that people can make from others' potentially offensive behaviors. In four studies (<em>n</em> = 1126), we tested whether being from a U.S. honor or dignity culture or the endorsement of these values was associated with hostile and nonhostile STIs, and whether honor and dignity influenced the relationship between STIs and behavioral intentions. In Study 1 and 2, honor culture participants made both types of STIs, whereas dignity culture participants only made hostile STIs. Study 3 revealed a positive association between individual honor endorsement and nonhostile STIs. In Study 4, we replicated most of these results and also found a positive association between hostile STIs and confrontation intentions in the honor (but not dignity) group. These results suggest that people from honor (vs. dignity) cultures tend to make rather simultaneous inferences of hostile and nonhostile traits from potentially offensive behaviors and making spontaneous hostile inferences can be more consequential for them. This research highlights the complexity of culture's influence on interpersonal processes and can have implications for diverse social contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104617"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140542516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To beckon or not to beckon: Testing a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment: A registered report 招还是不招?测试道德判断的因果-评价建模方法:注册报告
IF 3.5 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616
Cillian McHugh , Kathryn B. Francis , Jim A.C. Everett , Shane Timmons

Moral judgments are increasingly being understood as showing context dependent variability. A growing literature has identified a range of specific contextual factors (e.g., emotions, intentions) that can influence moral judgments in predictable ways. Integrating these diverse influences into a unified approach to understanding moral judgments remains a challenge. Recent work by Railton (2017) attempted to address this with a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment. In support of this model Railton presents evidence from novel variations of classic trolley type dilemmas. We present results from a pre-registered pilot study that highlight a significant confound and demonstrate that it likely influenced Railton's results. Building on this, our registered report presents a replication-extension of Railton's study, using larger more diverse samples, and more rigorous methods and materials, specifically controlling for potential confounds. We found that participants' judgments in sacrificial dilemmas are influenced by both direct personal force, and by whether harm occurs as a means or as a side-effect of action. We also show the relationship between a range of individual difference variables and responses to sacrificial moral dilemmas. Our results provide novel insights into the factors that influence people's moral judgments, and contribute to ongoing theoretical debates in moral psychology.

人们越来越认识到,道德判断显示出与环境相关的可变性。越来越多的文献发现,一系列特定的情境因素(如情感、意图)会以可预测的方式影响道德判断。将这些不同的影响因素整合到一个统一的方法中来理解道德判断仍然是一个挑战。Railton(2017)的最新研究试图通过一种道德判断的因果评价模型方法来解决这一问题。为了支持这一模型,Railton提出了经典手推车式困境的新变体证据。我们介绍了一项预先注册的试点研究结果,该研究强调了一个重要的混淆因素,并证明它很可能影响了 Railton 的研究结果。在此基础上,我们的注册报告对 Railton 的研究进行了复制和扩展,使用了更多的样本、更严格的方法和材料,特别是控制了潜在的混淆因素。我们发现,参与者在牺牲困境中的判断既受到直接个人力量的影响,也受到伤害是行动的手段还是副作用的影响。我们还展示了一系列个体差异变量与牺牲道德困境反应之间的关系。我们的研究结果为了解影响人们道德判断的因素提供了新的视角,并为道德心理学领域正在进行的理论辩论做出了贡献。
{"title":"To beckon or not to beckon: Testing a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment: A registered report","authors":"Cillian McHugh ,&nbsp;Kathryn B. Francis ,&nbsp;Jim A.C. Everett ,&nbsp;Shane Timmons","doi":"10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2024.104616","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Moral judgments are increasingly being understood as showing context dependent variability. A growing literature has identified a range of specific contextual factors (e.g., emotions, intentions) that can influence moral judgments in predictable ways. Integrating these diverse influences into a unified approach to understanding moral judgments remains a challenge. Recent work by Railton (2017) attempted to address this with a causal-evaluative modelling approach to moral judgment. In support of this model Railton presents evidence from novel variations of classic trolley type dilemmas. We present results from a pre-registered pilot study that highlight a significant confound and demonstrate that it likely influenced Railton's results. Building on this, our registered report presents a replication-extension of Railton's study, using larger more diverse samples, and more rigorous methods and materials, specifically controlling for potential confounds. We found that participants' judgments in sacrificial dilemmas are influenced by both direct personal force, and by whether harm occurs as a means or as a side-effect of action. We also show the relationship between a range of individual difference variables and responses to sacrificial moral dilemmas. Our results provide novel insights into the factors that influence people's moral judgments, and contribute to ongoing theoretical debates in moral psychology.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48441,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Social Psychology","volume":"113 ","pages":"Article 104616"},"PeriodicalIF":3.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022103124000283/pdfft?md5=64c886f733e92422f57027ea21a78ac0&pid=1-s2.0-S0022103124000283-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140536277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1