首页 > 最新文献

Communications Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Infants evaluate informativeness of evidence and predict causal events as revealed in theta oscillations and predictive looking 婴儿通过θ 振荡和预测性观察来评估证据的信息量并预测因果事件
Pub Date : 2024-08-20 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00131-3
Katarina Begus, Elizabeth Bonawitz
This study investigates 16-month-old infants’ sensitivity to the informativeness of evidence and its potential link to infants’ ability to draw accurate causal inferences and predict unfolding events. Employing concurrent EEG and eye tracking, data from 66 infants revealed significantly increased theta oscillatory activity when infants expected to see causally unconfounded evidence compared to confounded evidence, suggesting heightened cognitive engagement in anticipation of informative evidence. Crucially, this difference was more pronounced in the subset of infants who later made correct predictions, suggesting that they had correctly inferred the causal structure based on the evidence presented. This research sheds light on infants’ motivation to seek explanatory causal information, suggesting that even at 16 months, infants can strategically direct attention to situations conducive to acquiring informative evidence, potentially laying the groundwork for the impressive abilities of humans to rapidly acquire knowledge and develop causal theories of the world. 16-month-old infants showed heightened theta oscillations for informative versus uninformative causal evidence. This relationship was more pronounced in infants who subsequently made correct predictions based on this information.
本研究调查了 16 个月大婴儿对证据信息性的敏感性及其与婴儿做出准确因果推断和预测事件发展能力的潜在联系。通过同时使用脑电图和眼动追踪,66 名婴儿的数据显示,当婴儿预期看到无因果关系的证据时,θ 振荡活动比预期看到有因果关系的证据时显著增加,这表明婴儿在预期信息性证据时的认知参与程度有所提高。最重要的是,这种差异在后来做出正确预测的婴儿中更为明显,这表明他们根据所呈现的证据正确推断出了因果结构。这项研究揭示了婴儿寻求解释性因果信息的动机,表明即使是16个月大的婴儿也能有策略地将注意力引向有利于获取信息证据的情境,这可能为人类快速获取知识和发展世界因果理论的惊人能力奠定了基础。16 个月大的婴儿对有信息的因果证据和无信息的因果证据表现出更强的θ振荡。这种关系在随后根据这些信息做出正确预测的婴儿身上更为明显。
{"title":"Infants evaluate informativeness of evidence and predict causal events as revealed in theta oscillations and predictive looking","authors":"Katarina Begus, Elizabeth Bonawitz","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00131-3","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00131-3","url":null,"abstract":"This study investigates 16-month-old infants’ sensitivity to the informativeness of evidence and its potential link to infants’ ability to draw accurate causal inferences and predict unfolding events. Employing concurrent EEG and eye tracking, data from 66 infants revealed significantly increased theta oscillatory activity when infants expected to see causally unconfounded evidence compared to confounded evidence, suggesting heightened cognitive engagement in anticipation of informative evidence. Crucially, this difference was more pronounced in the subset of infants who later made correct predictions, suggesting that they had correctly inferred the causal structure based on the evidence presented. This research sheds light on infants’ motivation to seek explanatory causal information, suggesting that even at 16 months, infants can strategically direct attention to situations conducive to acquiring informative evidence, potentially laying the groundwork for the impressive abilities of humans to rapidly acquire knowledge and develop causal theories of the world. 16-month-old infants showed heightened theta oscillations for informative versus uninformative causal evidence. This relationship was more pronounced in infants who subsequently made correct predictions based on this information.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00131-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142013704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A theoretical framework for polarization as the gradual fragmentation of a divided society 两极分化是分裂社会逐渐分裂的理论框架
Pub Date : 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00125-1
Ana-Maria Bliuc, John M. Betts, Matteo Vergani, Ayoub Bouguettaya, Mioara Cristea
We propose a framework integrating insights from computational social science, political, and social psychology to explain how extreme polarization can occur in deeply divided societies. Extreme polarization in a society emerges through a dynamic and complex process where societal, group, and individual factors interact. Dissent at different levels of analysis represents the driver of this process, where societal-level ideological dissent divides society into opposing camps, each with contrasting collective narratives. Within these opposing camps, further dissent leads to the formation of splinter factions and radical cells—sub-groups with increasingly extreme views. At the group level, collective narratives underpinning group identity become more extreme as society fragments. At the individual level, this process involves the internalization of an extreme group narrative and norms sanctioning radical behavior. The intense bonding within these groups and the convergence of personal and group identities through identity fusion increase the likelihood of radical group behavior. A theoretical framework informed by computational social science and social psychology explains the process of polarization as the gradual fragmentation of a divided society.
我们提出了一个整合了计算社会科学、政治学和社会心理学见解的框架,以解释极端两极分化是如何在严重分裂的社会中出现的。社会中的极端两极分化是在社会、群体和个人因素相互作用的动态复杂过程中产生的。社会层面的意识形态分歧将社会划分为对立的阵营,每个阵营都有截然不同的集体叙事。在这些对立阵营中,进一步的异议会导致分裂派别和激进细胞--观点越来越极端的子群体--的形成。在群体层面,随着社会的分裂,支撑群体身份的集体叙事也变得更加极端。在个人层面,这一过程涉及极端群体叙事和认可激进行为的规范的内化。这些群体内部的紧密联系以及通过身份融合实现的个人身份与群体身份的趋同增加了激进群体行为的可能性。以计算社会科学和社会心理学为基础的理论框架将两极分化的过程解释为分裂社会的逐渐分裂。
{"title":"A theoretical framework for polarization as the gradual fragmentation of a divided society","authors":"Ana-Maria Bliuc, John M. Betts, Matteo Vergani, Ayoub Bouguettaya, Mioara Cristea","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00125-1","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00125-1","url":null,"abstract":"We propose a framework integrating insights from computational social science, political, and social psychology to explain how extreme polarization can occur in deeply divided societies. Extreme polarization in a society emerges through a dynamic and complex process where societal, group, and individual factors interact. Dissent at different levels of analysis represents the driver of this process, where societal-level ideological dissent divides society into opposing camps, each with contrasting collective narratives. Within these opposing camps, further dissent leads to the formation of splinter factions and radical cells—sub-groups with increasingly extreme views. At the group level, collective narratives underpinning group identity become more extreme as society fragments. At the individual level, this process involves the internalization of an extreme group narrative and norms sanctioning radical behavior. The intense bonding within these groups and the convergence of personal and group identities through identity fusion increase the likelihood of radical group behavior. A theoretical framework informed by computational social science and social psychology explains the process of polarization as the gradual fragmentation of a divided society.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00125-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141986138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Media literacy tips promoting reliable news improve discernment and enhance trust in traditional media 宣传可靠新闻的媒体扫盲小贴士可提高辨别力,增强对传统媒体的信任
Pub Date : 2024-08-14 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00121-5
Sacha Altay, Andrea De Angelis, Emma Hoes
Media literacy tips typically encourage people to be skeptical of the news despite the small prevalence of false news in Western democracies. Would such tips be effective if they promoted trust in true news instead? A pre-registered experiment (N = 3919, US) showed that Skepticism-enhancing tips, Trust-inducing tips, and a mix of both tips, increased participants’ sharing and accuracy discernment. The Trust-inducing tips boosted true news sharing and acceptance, the Skepticism-enhancing tips hindered false news sharing and acceptance, while the Mixed tips did both. Yet, the effects of the tips were more alike than different, with very similar effect sizes across conditions for true and false news. We experimentally manipulated the proportion of true and false news participants were exposed to. The Trust and Skepticism tips were most effective when participants were exposed to equal proportions of true and false news, while the Mixed tips were most effective when exposed to 75% of true news - the most realistic proportion. Moreover, the Trust-inducing tips increased trust in traditional media. Overall, we show that to be most effective, media literacy tips should aim both to foster skepticism towards false news and to promote trust in true news. Media literacy tips promoting trust in true news, skepticism of false news, or a mix of both, were all effective in improving discernment between true and false news stories relative to a control condition without tips.
尽管虚假新闻在西方民主国家并不普遍,但媒体扫盲提示通常鼓励人们对新闻持怀疑态度。如果这些提示能促进人们对真实新闻的信任,那么它们会有效吗?一项预先登记的实验(N = 3919,美国)显示,提高怀疑度的提示、增加信任度的提示以及这两种提示的混合使用,都提高了参与者的分享率和准确性鉴别力。信任诱导提示促进了真实新闻的分享和接受,怀疑主义增强提示阻碍了虚假新闻的分享和接受,而混合提示则两者兼而有之。然而,这些提示的效果与其说是不同的,不如说是相似的,在不同条件下,真实新闻和虚假新闻的效果大小非常相似。我们通过实验操纵了参与者接触到的真假新闻的比例。当参与者接触到的真假新闻比例相同时,信任和怀疑提示的效果最好;而当参与者接触到 75% 的真假新闻时(这是最真实的比例),混合提示的效果最好。此外,"信任诱导 "提示提高了人们对传统媒体的信任度。总之,我们的研究表明,媒体扫盲小贴士要想取得最大的效果,就应该既培养人们对虚假新闻的怀疑态度,又提高人们对真实新闻的信任度。相对于没有提示的对照条件,促进对真实新闻的信任、对虚假新闻的怀疑或两者兼而有之的媒体扫盲提示都能有效提高对真假新闻的辨别能力。
{"title":"Media literacy tips promoting reliable news improve discernment and enhance trust in traditional media","authors":"Sacha Altay, Andrea De Angelis, Emma Hoes","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00121-5","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00121-5","url":null,"abstract":"Media literacy tips typically encourage people to be skeptical of the news despite the small prevalence of false news in Western democracies. Would such tips be effective if they promoted trust in true news instead? A pre-registered experiment (N = 3919, US) showed that Skepticism-enhancing tips, Trust-inducing tips, and a mix of both tips, increased participants’ sharing and accuracy discernment. The Trust-inducing tips boosted true news sharing and acceptance, the Skepticism-enhancing tips hindered false news sharing and acceptance, while the Mixed tips did both. Yet, the effects of the tips were more alike than different, with very similar effect sizes across conditions for true and false news. We experimentally manipulated the proportion of true and false news participants were exposed to. The Trust and Skepticism tips were most effective when participants were exposed to equal proportions of true and false news, while the Mixed tips were most effective when exposed to 75% of true news - the most realistic proportion. Moreover, the Trust-inducing tips increased trust in traditional media. Overall, we show that to be most effective, media literacy tips should aim both to foster skepticism towards false news and to promote trust in true news. Media literacy tips promoting trust in true news, skepticism of false news, or a mix of both, were all effective in improving discernment between true and false news stories relative to a control condition without tips.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00121-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141986139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Both partners’ negative emotion drives aggression during couples’ conflict 在夫妻冲突中,伴侣双方的负面情绪都是攻击行为的驱动因素
Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00122-4
Annah G. McCurry, Robert C. May, David I. Donaldson
Researchers examining conflict between intimate partners believe that the experience and expression of emotion drives aggressive behaviour. Intra-personally, increases in negative affect make aggression more likely. Inter-personally, theoretical models suggest that each individuals’ perception of their partners’ emotion also influences aggression, potentially creating a Violence Escalation Cycle. Here, using a lab-based aggression task across a primary study (n = 104, number of trials = 3095) and a replication (n = 58, number of trials = 3167), we show that both intra- and inter-personal experiences of negative emotion predict reactive aggression within couples, revealing retaliation but not escalation. Critically, analyses of facial affect reveal that prototypic displays of negative emotions have a compounding effect, leading to dramatic changes in aggression depending on whether one, both, or neither partner expressed negative emotion. We propose a mechanism by which temporal delays (i.e., experimentally imposed forced breaks) reduce aggression by decreasing negative emotional arousal and limiting impulsive action. Our results show that both forced breaks and elective breaks (i.e., extra participant-initiated extensions of the forced break time) reduce aggression, providing exciting evidence that interventions focused on preventing impulsive action when people are in a provoked state can reduce aggression within couples. Couples competing in a reaction time task where they can play loud sounds to their partner are more aggressive when negative facial affect is expressed. Couples retaliate, but introducing short breaks reduces negative affect and aggressive behaviour.
研究亲密伴侣间冲突的研究人员认为,情绪的体验和表达是攻击行为的驱动力。在人与人之间,负面情绪的增加更有可能导致攻击行为。从人际关系来看,理论模型表明,每个人对伴侣情绪的感知也会影响攻击行为,从而可能形成暴力升级循环。在这里,我们通过一项基于实验室的攻击任务的初步研究(n = 104,试验次数 = 3095)和一项重复研究(n = 58,试验次数 = 3167)表明,人内和人际间的负面情绪体验都能预测夫妻间的反应性攻击,揭示了报复而非升级。重要的是,对面部情绪的分析表明,负面情绪的原型显示具有复合效应,会导致攻击性的巨大变化,这取决于一方、双方或双方都没有表达负面情绪。我们提出了一种机制,即时间延迟(即实验中施加的强制中断)通过降低负面情绪唤醒和限制冲动行为来减少攻击性。我们的研究结果表明,强制休息时间和选择性休息时间(即参与者主动延长强制休息时间)都能减少攻击行为,这为我们提供了令人兴奋的证据,证明当人们处于被激怒状态时,专注于防止冲动行为的干预措施可以减少情侣间的攻击行为。在一项反应时间任务中,参加比赛的情侣可以向其伴侣播放响亮的声音,当伴侣表达负面面部情绪时,情侣的攻击性会更强。情侣们会进行报复,但短暂的休息会减少负面情绪和攻击行为。
{"title":"Both partners’ negative emotion drives aggression during couples’ conflict","authors":"Annah G. McCurry, Robert C. May, David I. Donaldson","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00122-4","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00122-4","url":null,"abstract":"Researchers examining conflict between intimate partners believe that the experience and expression of emotion drives aggressive behaviour. Intra-personally, increases in negative affect make aggression more likely. Inter-personally, theoretical models suggest that each individuals’ perception of their partners’ emotion also influences aggression, potentially creating a Violence Escalation Cycle. Here, using a lab-based aggression task across a primary study (n = 104, number of trials = 3095) and a replication (n = 58, number of trials = 3167), we show that both intra- and inter-personal experiences of negative emotion predict reactive aggression within couples, revealing retaliation but not escalation. Critically, analyses of facial affect reveal that prototypic displays of negative emotions have a compounding effect, leading to dramatic changes in aggression depending on whether one, both, or neither partner expressed negative emotion. We propose a mechanism by which temporal delays (i.e., experimentally imposed forced breaks) reduce aggression by decreasing negative emotional arousal and limiting impulsive action. Our results show that both forced breaks and elective breaks (i.e., extra participant-initiated extensions of the forced break time) reduce aggression, providing exciting evidence that interventions focused on preventing impulsive action when people are in a provoked state can reduce aggression within couples. Couples competing in a reaction time task where they can play loud sounds to their partner are more aggressive when negative facial affect is expressed. Couples retaliate, but introducing short breaks reduces negative affect and aggressive behaviour.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00122-4.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141968504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Robots are both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally 当机器人受到故意伤害时,既会被拟人化,也会被非人化
Pub Date : 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00116-2
Marieke S. Wieringa, Barbara C. N. Müller, Gijsbert Bijlstra, Tibor Bosse
The harm-made mind phenomenon implies that witnessing intentional harm towards agents with ambiguous minds, such as robots, leads to augmented mind perception in these agents. We conducted two replications of previous work on this effect and extended it by testing if robots that detect and simulate emotions elicit a stronger harm-made mind effect than robots that do not. Additionally, we explored if someone is perceived as less prosocial when harming a robot compared to treating it kindly. The harm made mind-effect was replicated: participants attributed a higher capacity to experience pain to the robot when it was harmed, compared to when it was not harmed. We did not find evidence that this effect was influenced by the robot’s ability to detect and simulate emotions. There were significant but conflicting direct and indirect effects of harm on the perception of mind in the robot: while harm had a positive indirect effect on mind perception in the robot through the perceived capacity for pain, the direct effect of harm on mind perception was negative. This suggests that robots are both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally. Additionally, the results showed that someone is perceived as less prosocial when harming a robot compared to treating it kindly. A replication of the harm-made mind effect finds conflicting effects of harm on the perception of mind in robots —robots were both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally while the harmer was perceived as less prosocial.
伤害心理现象意味着,目睹对具有模糊心理的代理(如机器人)的故意伤害会导致这些代理的心理感知增强。我们对以前关于这种效应的研究进行了两次复制,并通过测试检测和模拟情感的机器人是否会比不检测和模拟情感的机器人引发更强烈的伤害心理效应来扩展这种效应。此外,我们还探讨了与善待机器人相比,伤害机器人的人是否会被认为不那么亲社会。伤害心理效应得到了验证:与未受到伤害时相比,当机器人受到伤害时,参与者会认为它有更强的疼痛体验能力。我们没有发现证据表明这种效应受到机器人检测和模拟情绪能力的影响。伤害对机器人心灵感知的直接和间接影响都很明显,但却相互矛盾:伤害通过对疼痛的感知能力对机器人的心灵感知产生了积极的间接影响,但伤害对心灵感知的直接影响却是消极的。这表明,当机器人受到有意伤害时,既会被拟人化,也会被非人性化。此外,研究结果表明,与善待机器人相比,伤害机器人的人被认为更不亲近社会。对 "伤害造就心灵效应 "的复制发现,伤害对机器人心灵感知的影响是相互矛盾的--机器人在受到故意伤害时既会被拟人化,也会被非人性化,而伤害者则被视为较不亲近社会的人。
{"title":"Robots are both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally","authors":"Marieke S. Wieringa, Barbara C. N. Müller, Gijsbert Bijlstra, Tibor Bosse","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00116-2","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00116-2","url":null,"abstract":"The harm-made mind phenomenon implies that witnessing intentional harm towards agents with ambiguous minds, such as robots, leads to augmented mind perception in these agents. We conducted two replications of previous work on this effect and extended it by testing if robots that detect and simulate emotions elicit a stronger harm-made mind effect than robots that do not. Additionally, we explored if someone is perceived as less prosocial when harming a robot compared to treating it kindly. The harm made mind-effect was replicated: participants attributed a higher capacity to experience pain to the robot when it was harmed, compared to when it was not harmed. We did not find evidence that this effect was influenced by the robot’s ability to detect and simulate emotions. There were significant but conflicting direct and indirect effects of harm on the perception of mind in the robot: while harm had a positive indirect effect on mind perception in the robot through the perceived capacity for pain, the direct effect of harm on mind perception was negative. This suggests that robots are both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally. Additionally, the results showed that someone is perceived as less prosocial when harming a robot compared to treating it kindly. A replication of the harm-made mind effect finds conflicting effects of harm on the perception of mind in robots —robots were both anthropomorphized and dehumanized when harmed intentionally while the harmer was perceived as less prosocial.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00116-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141968582","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Online communities come with real-world consequences for individuals and societies 网络社区给个人和社会带来现实世界的影响
Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00112-6
Atte Oksanen, Magdalena Celuch, Reetta Oksa, Iina Savolainen
Online communities have become a central part of the internet. Understanding what motivates users to join these communities, and how they affect them and others, spans various psychological domains, including organizational psychology, political and social psychology, and clinical and health psychology. We focus on online communities that are exemplary for three domains: work, hate, and addictions. We review the risks that emerge from these online communities but also recognize the opportunities that work and behavioral addiction communities present for groups and individuals. With the continued evolution of online spheres, online communities are likely to have an increasingly significant role in all spheres of life, ranging from personal to professional and from individual to societal. Psychological research provides critical insights into understanding the formation of online communities, and the implications for individuals and society. To counteract risks, it needs to identify opportunities for prevention and support. Online Communities play an increasing role in online behaviour and affect offline lives. Psychological research on online work communities, hate communities, and communities dedicated to behavioural addictions highlights risks and – in some cases - opportunities.
在线社区已成为互联网的核心部分。了解是什么促使用户加入这些社区,以及这些社区如何影响他们和其他人,这涉及多个心理学领域,包括组织心理学、政治和社会心理学,以及临床和健康心理学。我们将重点放在工作、仇恨和成瘾这三个领域的网络社区上。我们回顾了这些网络社区带来的风险,同时也认识到工作和行为成瘾社区为团体和个人带来的机遇。随着网络领域的不断发展,网络社区可能会在从个人到职业、从个人到社会的所有生活领域中发挥越来越重要的作用。心理学研究为了解网络社区的形成及其对个人和社会的影响提供了重要的见解。为了抵御风险,需要确定预防和支持的机会。网络社区在网上行为中发挥着越来越大的作用,并影响着网下生活。对网上工作社区、仇恨社区和行为成瘾社区的心理学研究凸显了风险,在某些情况下也凸显了机遇。
{"title":"Online communities come with real-world consequences for individuals and societies","authors":"Atte Oksanen, Magdalena Celuch, Reetta Oksa, Iina Savolainen","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00112-6","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00112-6","url":null,"abstract":"Online communities have become a central part of the internet. Understanding what motivates users to join these communities, and how they affect them and others, spans various psychological domains, including organizational psychology, political and social psychology, and clinical and health psychology. We focus on online communities that are exemplary for three domains: work, hate, and addictions. We review the risks that emerge from these online communities but also recognize the opportunities that work and behavioral addiction communities present for groups and individuals. With the continued evolution of online spheres, online communities are likely to have an increasingly significant role in all spheres of life, ranging from personal to professional and from individual to societal. Psychological research provides critical insights into understanding the formation of online communities, and the implications for individuals and society. To counteract risks, it needs to identify opportunities for prevention and support. Online Communities play an increasing role in online behaviour and affect offline lives. Psychological research on online work communities, hate communities, and communities dedicated to behavioural addictions highlights risks and – in some cases - opportunities.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00112-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141968583","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The rising entropy of English in the attention economy 注意力经济时代英语熵的上升
Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00117-1
Charlie Pilgrim, Weisi Guo, Thomas T. Hills
We present evidence that the word entropy of American English has been rising steadily since around 1900. We also find differences in word entropy between media categories, with short-form media such as news and magazines having higher entropy than long-form media, and social media feeds having higher entropy still. To explain these results we develop an ecological model of the attention economy that combines ideas from Zipf’s law and information foraging. In this model, media consumers maximize information utility rate taking into account the costs of information search, while media producers adapt to technologies that reduce search costs, driving them to generate higher entropy content in increasingly shorter formats. American English has become more information dense over the last 100 years, likely driven by competition for human attention.
我们提出的证据表明,自 1900 年左右以来,美式英语的词熵一直在稳步上升。我们还发现了媒体类别之间的词熵差异,新闻和杂志等短篇媒体的词熵高于长篇媒体,而社交媒体的词熵仍然更高。为了解释这些结果,我们结合齐普夫定律和信息觅食理论,建立了一个注意力经济生态模型。在这个模型中,媒体消费者在考虑到信息搜索成本的情况下最大化信息效用率,而媒体生产者则适应降低搜索成本的技术,促使他们以越来越短的格式生产熵值更高的内容。在过去的 100 年里,美式英语的信息密度越来越高,这很可能是由于对人类注意力的争夺所致。
{"title":"The rising entropy of English in the attention economy","authors":"Charlie Pilgrim, Weisi Guo, Thomas T. Hills","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00117-1","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00117-1","url":null,"abstract":"We present evidence that the word entropy of American English has been rising steadily since around 1900. We also find differences in word entropy between media categories, with short-form media such as news and magazines having higher entropy than long-form media, and social media feeds having higher entropy still. To explain these results we develop an ecological model of the attention economy that combines ideas from Zipf’s law and information foraging. In this model, media consumers maximize information utility rate taking into account the costs of information search, while media producers adapt to technologies that reduce search costs, driving them to generate higher entropy content in increasingly shorter formats. American English has become more information dense over the last 100 years, likely driven by competition for human attention.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00117-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141968585","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Integrated theory of false insights and beliefs under psychedelics 迷幻药作用下的错误认识和信念的综合理论
Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00120-6
H. T. McGovern, H. J. Grimmer, M. K. Doss, B. T. Hutchinson, C. Timmermann, A. Lyon, P. R. Corlett, R. E. Laukkonen
Psychedelics are recognised for their potential to re-orient beliefs. We propose a model of how psychedelics can, in some cases, lead to false insights and thus false beliefs. We first review experimental work on laboratory-based false insights and false memories. We then connect this to insights and belief formation under psychedelics using the active inference framework. We propose that subjective and brain-based alterations caused by psychedelics increases the quantity and subjective intensity of insights and thence beliefs, including false ones. We offer directions for future research in minimising the risk of false and potentially harmful beliefs arising from psychedelics. Ultimately, knowing how psychedelics may facilitate false insights and beliefs is crucial if we are to optimally leverage their therapeutic potential. The neurobiological action of psychedelics on the brain may increase the intensity of the experience of insight. Psychedelics can thus lead to flexible updating of beliefs, critically including the adoption of false beliefs.
迷幻药被认为具有重新定位信念的潜力。我们提出了一个模型,说明迷幻药在某些情况下如何会导致错误的洞察力,进而导致错误的信念。我们首先回顾了基于实验室的错误认识和错误记忆的实验工作。然后,我们利用主动推理框架将其与迷幻药作用下的洞察力和信念形成联系起来。我们提出,迷幻药引起的主观和大脑变化会增加洞察力的数量和主观强度,进而增加信念,包括错误信念。我们为未来的研究提供了方向,以最大限度地降低迷幻药导致错误和潜在有害信念的风险。归根结底,如果我们要以最佳方式发挥迷幻药的治疗潜力,那么了解迷幻药是如何促进产生错误见解和信念的至关重要。迷幻药对大脑的神经生物学作用可能会增加洞察体验的强度。因此,迷幻药可能会导致信念的灵活更新,关键是包括采用错误的信念。
{"title":"An Integrated theory of false insights and beliefs under psychedelics","authors":"H. T. McGovern, H. J. Grimmer, M. K. Doss, B. T. Hutchinson, C. Timmermann, A. Lyon, P. R. Corlett, R. E. Laukkonen","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00120-6","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00120-6","url":null,"abstract":"Psychedelics are recognised for their potential to re-orient beliefs. We propose a model of how psychedelics can, in some cases, lead to false insights and thus false beliefs. We first review experimental work on laboratory-based false insights and false memories. We then connect this to insights and belief formation under psychedelics using the active inference framework. We propose that subjective and brain-based alterations caused by psychedelics increases the quantity and subjective intensity of insights and thence beliefs, including false ones. We offer directions for future research in minimising the risk of false and potentially harmful beliefs arising from psychedelics. Ultimately, knowing how psychedelics may facilitate false insights and beliefs is crucial if we are to optimally leverage their therapeutic potential. The neurobiological action of psychedelics on the brain may increase the intensity of the experience of insight. Psychedelics can thus lead to flexible updating of beliefs, critically including the adoption of false beliefs.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00120-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141968584","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anchor objects drive realism while diagnostic objects drive categorization in GAN generated scenes 在 GAN 生成的场景中,锚点对象推动真实感,而诊断对象推动分类
Pub Date : 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00119-z
Aylin Kallmayer, Melissa L.-H. Võ
Our visual surroundings are highly complex. Despite this, we understand and navigate them effortlessly. This requires transforming incoming sensory information into representations that not only span low- to high-level visual features (e.g., edges, object parts, objects), but likely also reflect co-occurrence statistics of objects in real-world scenes. Here, so-called anchor objects are defined as being highly predictive of the location and identity of frequently co-occuring (usually smaller) objects, derived from object clustering statistics in real-world scenes, while so-called diagnostic objects are predictive of the larger semantic context (i.e., scene category). Across two studies (N1 = 50, N2 = 44), we investigate which of these properties underlie scene understanding across two dimensions – realism and categorisation – using scenes generated from Generative Adversarial Networks (GANs) which naturally vary along these dimensions. We show that anchor objects and mainly high-level features extracted from a range of pre-trained deep neural networks (DNNs) drove realism both at first glance and after initial processing. Categorisation performance was mainly determined by diagnostic objects, regardless of realism, at first glance and after initial processing. Our results are testament to the visual system’s ability to pick up on reliable, category specific sources of information that are flexible towards disturbances across the visual feature-hierarchy. Human observers rate Generative Adversarial Network scenes as more realistic if they contain appropriate anchor objects, while scene categorization relies on diagnostic objects.
我们周围的视觉环境非常复杂。尽管如此,我们仍能毫不费力地理解和浏览它们。这就需要将传入的感官信息转化为表征,这些表征不仅涵盖从低级到高级的视觉特征(如边缘、物体部分、物体),而且还可能反映真实世界场景中物体的共现统计。在这里,所谓的锚定对象被定义为对频繁出现的(通常是较小)对象的位置和身份具有较高的预测性,这是从真实世界场景中的对象聚类统计中得出的,而所谓的诊断对象则是对更大的语义背景(即场景类别)具有预测性。通过两项研究(N1 = 50,N2 = 44),我们利用生成对抗网络(GANs)生成的场景(这些场景自然会在这些维度上发生变化),调查了这些属性中哪些属性是场景理解在两个维度(逼真度和分类)上的基础。我们的研究表明,锚定对象和主要从一系列预先训练的深度神经网络(DNN)中提取的高级特征在第一眼和初步处理后都能推动逼真度的提高。无论逼真度如何,在第一眼和初步处理后,分类性能主要由诊断对象决定。我们的研究结果证明,视觉系统有能力捕捉可靠的、特定类别的信息源,这些信息源对整个视觉特征层的干扰具有灵活性。如果生成对抗网络场景包含适当的锚定对象,那么人类观察者就会认为这些场景更加逼真,而场景分类则依赖于诊断对象。
{"title":"Anchor objects drive realism while diagnostic objects drive categorization in GAN generated scenes","authors":"Aylin Kallmayer, Melissa L.-H. Võ","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00119-z","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00119-z","url":null,"abstract":"Our visual surroundings are highly complex. Despite this, we understand and navigate them effortlessly. This requires transforming incoming sensory information into representations that not only span low- to high-level visual features (e.g., edges, object parts, objects), but likely also reflect co-occurrence statistics of objects in real-world scenes. Here, so-called anchor objects are defined as being highly predictive of the location and identity of frequently co-occuring (usually smaller) objects, derived from object clustering statistics in real-world scenes, while so-called diagnostic objects are predictive of the larger semantic context (i.e., scene category). Across two studies (N1 = 50, N2 = 44), we investigate which of these properties underlie scene understanding across two dimensions – realism and categorisation – using scenes generated from Generative Adversarial Networks (GANs) which naturally vary along these dimensions. We show that anchor objects and mainly high-level features extracted from a range of pre-trained deep neural networks (DNNs) drove realism both at first glance and after initial processing. Categorisation performance was mainly determined by diagnostic objects, regardless of realism, at first glance and after initial processing. Our results are testament to the visual system’s ability to pick up on reliable, category specific sources of information that are flexible towards disturbances across the visual feature-hierarchy. Human observers rate Generative Adversarial Network scenes as more realistic if they contain appropriate anchor objects, while scene categorization relies on diagnostic objects.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00119-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141800578","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between time perspective and life satisfaction across adulthood 时间观念与成年期生活满意度之间的纵向联系
Pub Date : 2024-07-20 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00118-0
Maria Wirth, Markus Wettstein, Klaus Rothermund
Time perspective is an important predictor of well-being. How time is represented, is itself subject to developmental change. A time perspective dominated by the future is increasingly replaced by one focused on the present and past as remaining lifetime decreases. These age-related changes supposedly are associated with higher subjective well-being. Previous studies yielded heterogeneous results. However, these studies mostly investigated one dimension of time perspective and did not include younger and/or middle-aged adults. Thus, we investigated how changes in four facets of time perspective (past-orientation, concreteness of future time, obsolescence, and attitudes towards finitude) were related to changes in life and domain-specific satisfaction and if these relations were moderated by age. We used 10-year longitudinal data from an age-diverse sample comprising 459 participants (30–80 years). Concreteness was most consistently related to satisfaction. Individuals with overall higher concreteness reported higher life satisfaction and higher life satisfaction was reported on measurement occasions with higher concreteness. An age moderation was only found for satisfaction with mental fitness. Among younger but not older adults, satisfaction with mental fitness was higher on measurement occasions with higher concreteness. Our study provides a deeper understanding of the relation between time perspective and well-being across adulthood. Across occasions and individuals, perceiving the future more concretely was associated with greater life satisfaction for adults aged 30 to 80 years. Age moderated this association in the domain of mental fitness with stronger associations for younger adults.
时间观念是幸福感的重要预测因素。时间的表现方式本身也会随着发展而变化。随着剩余寿命的减少,以未来为主导的时间观逐渐被以现在和过去为主导的时间观所取代。据称,这些与年龄有关的变化与主观幸福感的提高有关。以往的研究得出了不同的结果。然而,这些研究大多只调查了时间观的一个维度,而且没有将年轻人和/或中年人包括在内。因此,我们研究了时间观的四个方面(过去取向、未来时间的具体性、陈旧性和对有限性的态度)的变化与生活和特定领域满意度变化的关系,以及这些关系是否受年龄的调节。我们使用了由 459 名参与者(30-80 岁)组成的不同年龄样本的 10 年纵向数据。具体性与满意度的关系最为一致。总体具体程度较高的个体报告的生活满意度较高,在具体程度较高的测量场合报告的生活满意度也较高。只有在心理健康满意度方面发现了年龄调节。在年轻人中,精神健康满意度在具体程度较高的测量场合较高,而在老年人中则不然。我们的研究加深了人们对时间视角与成年期幸福感之间关系的理解。对于 30 至 80 岁的成年人来说,在不同场合和不同个体中,对未来的感知更具体与更高的生活满意度相关。在心理健康领域,年龄调节了这种关联,年轻成年人的关联性更强。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between time perspective and life satisfaction across adulthood","authors":"Maria Wirth, Markus Wettstein, Klaus Rothermund","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00118-0","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00118-0","url":null,"abstract":"Time perspective is an important predictor of well-being. How time is represented, is itself subject to developmental change. A time perspective dominated by the future is increasingly replaced by one focused on the present and past as remaining lifetime decreases. These age-related changes supposedly are associated with higher subjective well-being. Previous studies yielded heterogeneous results. However, these studies mostly investigated one dimension of time perspective and did not include younger and/or middle-aged adults. Thus, we investigated how changes in four facets of time perspective (past-orientation, concreteness of future time, obsolescence, and attitudes towards finitude) were related to changes in life and domain-specific satisfaction and if these relations were moderated by age. We used 10-year longitudinal data from an age-diverse sample comprising 459 participants (30–80 years). Concreteness was most consistently related to satisfaction. Individuals with overall higher concreteness reported higher life satisfaction and higher life satisfaction was reported on measurement occasions with higher concreteness. An age moderation was only found for satisfaction with mental fitness. Among younger but not older adults, satisfaction with mental fitness was higher on measurement occasions with higher concreteness. Our study provides a deeper understanding of the relation between time perspective and well-being across adulthood. Across occasions and individuals, perceiving the future more concretely was associated with greater life satisfaction for adults aged 30 to 80 years. Age moderated this association in the domain of mental fitness with stronger associations for younger adults.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00118-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141819374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Communications Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1