Pub Date : 2024-05-10DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103741
Zenan Chen, Bin Shao
In modern Chinese, an object can precede or follow a quantifier (numeral + verbal classifier) when they co-occur after a verb, which is supposedly affected by multiple factors. Within a usage-based probabilistic variationist framework, this study defines the above ordering as the alternation of the Event-quantifying Construction and examines ten potential variables that probabilistically constrain the construction alternation, using three multivariate methods. Results indicated that six variables significantly affect the selection of the two variants, i.e., “Quantifier-first construction” and “Object-first construction”, including the animacy, definiteness, givenness, and pronominality of objects, as well as the length difference between objects and quantifiers and the type of verbal classifiers. The two variants differ in the values of these variables, which may result from the postverbal constraint, the end-focus principle, and the end-weight principle in Chinese. However, these distinctions in the formal or semantic aspects will be neutralized in natural communication, thereby allowing the alternation of this construction.
{"title":"Alternation in the Chinese Event-quantifying Construction: A multivariate approach","authors":"Zenan Chen, Bin Shao","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103741","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103741","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In modern Chinese, an object can precede or follow a quantifier (numeral + verbal classifier) when they co-occur after a verb, which is supposedly affected by multiple factors. Within a usage-based probabilistic variationist framework, this study defines the above ordering as the alternation of the Event-quantifying Construction and examines ten potential variables that probabilistically constrain the construction alternation, using three multivariate methods. Results indicated that six variables significantly affect the selection of the two variants, i.e., “Quantifier-first construction” and “Object-first construction”, including the animacy, definiteness, givenness, and pronominality of objects, as well as the length difference between objects and quantifiers and the type of verbal classifiers. The two variants differ in the values of these variables, which may result from the postverbal constraint, the end-focus principle, and the end-weight principle in Chinese. However, these distinctions in the formal or semantic aspects will be neutralized in natural communication, thereby allowing the alternation of this construction.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"305 ","pages":"Article 103741"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140906763","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-04DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103736
Jiajia Su
This article investigates the second language (L2) acquisition of classifier reduplication in Chinese by Korean speakers and English speakers. Unlike the individuation function of simple classifiers, reduplicated classifiers express plurality. Yi-Cl-Cl encodes abundant plural and is associated with [plural, abundant] features, while Cl-Cl encodes distributive plural and is associated with [plural, distributive] features. Thirty-two English-speaking and thirty-five Korean-speaking learners of Chinese at advanced and intermediate Chinese proficiency levels were tested using a grammaticality judgment task and a truth value judgment task. The group and individual results show that, though great acquisition difficulties were encountered, both English and Korean speakers can eventually acquire the features associated with classifier reduplication. This empirical study contributes to the L2 theory of feature reassembly, by looking into the process of feature reassembly when the linguistic property is morphologically and semantically complex, and form-meaning transparency is low. This study also provides new data on the L2 acquisition of classifiers and plurality.
{"title":"L2 acquisition of classifier reduplication in Chinese","authors":"Jiajia Su","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103736","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103736","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This article investigates the second language (L2) acquisition of classifier reduplication in Chinese by Korean speakers and English speakers. Unlike the individuation function of simple classifiers, reduplicated classifiers express plurality. <em>Yi-Cl-Cl</em> encodes abundant plural and is associated with [plural, abundant] features, while <em>Cl-Cl</em> encodes distributive plural and is associated with [plural, distributive] features. Thirty-two English-speaking and thirty-five Korean-speaking learners of Chinese at advanced and intermediate Chinese proficiency levels were tested using a grammaticality judgment task and a truth value judgment task. The group and individual results show that, though great acquisition difficulties were encountered, both English and Korean speakers can eventually acquire the features associated with classifier reduplication. This empirical study contributes to the L2 theory of feature reassembly, by looking into the process of feature reassembly when the linguistic property is morphologically and semantically complex, and form-meaning transparency is low. This study also provides new data on the L2 acquisition of classifiers and plurality.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"305 ","pages":"Article 103736"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0024384124000652/pdfft?md5=4ef010333051d5fec713c6f4048c7481&pid=1-s2.0-S0024384124000652-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140822757","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Scholarly opinions vary on what language is, how it evolved, and from where or what it evolved. Long considered uniquely human, today scholars argue for evolutionary continuity between human language and animal communication systems. But while it is generally recognized that language is an evolving communication system, scholars continue to debate from which species language evolved, and what behavioral and cognitive features are the precursors to human language. To understand the nature of protolanguage, some look for homologs in gene functionality, brain areas, or anatomical structures such as the supralaryngeal vocal tract; others point toward primates, their gestural, vocal, multimodal, and in later evolving hominins also their pantomimic communication systems; and still others draw parallels between the musicality that characterizes language and the pitch found in the numerous sounds produced by animals. Accordingly, protolanguage theories today are multiple and diverse, and protolanguages might have also been diverse. This special issue on Evolving (Proto)Language/s for Lingua bundles several of the protolanguage theories that were put forward at the sixth edition of the Ways to Protolanguage conference series, held at the Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation in Lisbon, in 2019. Not aimed at surveying all the different ways there are to conceptualize, study, and model protolanguage/s, this issue provides interested readers with good overviews on the role played in current theorizing on protolanguage/s by (paleo)anthropology, genetics, physiology, developmental, evolutionary, ecological, and pragmatic research lines.
关于什么是语言、语言是如何进化的、语言是从哪里或从什么地方进化来的,学者们众说纷纭。长期以来,语言一直被认为是人类独有的,如今,学者们认为人类语言和动物交流系统在进化上具有连续性。尽管人们普遍认为语言是一种不断进化的交流系统,但学者们仍在争论语言是从哪个物种进化而来,以及人类语言的前身具有哪些行为和认知特征。为了理解原语言的本质,一些人从基因功能、大脑区域或解剖结构(如喉上声道)中寻找同源物;另一些人则将目光投向灵长类动物,它们的手势、发声、多模态,以及后来进化的类人猿的拟声交流系统;还有一些人将语言的音乐性特征与动物发出的无数声音中的音高相提并论。因此,今天的原语言理论是多种多样的,原语言也可能是多种多样的。本期《Lingua》特刊的主题是 "演变中的(原)语言"(Evolving (Proto)Language/s for Lingua),收录了2019年在里斯本卡洛斯特-古尔班基安基金会(Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation)举行的第六届 "原语言之路"(Ways to Protolanguage)系列会议上提出的几种原语言理论。本期并不旨在对原语概念化、研究和建模的所有不同方法进行调查,而是为感兴趣的读者提供有关(古)人类学、遗传学、生理学、发展学、进化论、生态学和语用学等研究方向在当前原语理论研究中所发挥的作用的概览。
{"title":"Introduction to Evolving (Proto)Language/s","authors":"Nathalie Gontier , Monika Boruta Żywiczyńska , Sverker Johansson , Lorraine McCune","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103740","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103740","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Scholarly opinions vary on what language is, how it evolved, and from where or what it evolved. Long considered uniquely human, today scholars argue for evolutionary continuity between human language and animal communication systems. But while it is generally recognized that language is an evolving communication system, scholars continue to debate from which species language evolved, and what behavioral and cognitive features are the precursors to human language. To understand the nature of protolanguage, some look for homologs in gene functionality, brain areas, or anatomical structures such as the supralaryngeal vocal tract; others point toward primates, their gestural, vocal, multimodal, and in later evolving hominins also their pantomimic communication systems; and still others draw parallels between the musicality that characterizes language and the pitch found in the numerous sounds produced by animals. Accordingly, protolanguage theories today are multiple and diverse, and protolanguages might have also been diverse. This special issue on Evolving (Proto)Language/s for Lingua bundles several of the protolanguage theories that were put forward at the sixth edition of the Ways to Protolanguage conference series, held at the Calouste Gulbenkian Foundation in Lisbon, in 2019. Not aimed at surveying all the different ways there are to conceptualize, study, and model protolanguage/s, this issue provides interested readers with good overviews on the role played in current theorizing on protolanguage/s by (paleo)anthropology, genetics, physiology, developmental, evolutionary, ecological, and pragmatic research lines.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"305 ","pages":"Article 103740"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140815468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-25DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103737
Warda Nejjari, Roeland van Hout, Marinel Gerritsen, Brigitte Planken
We investigated the extent to which responses (N = 6617) by four L2 English listener groups (The Netherlands: n = 1701; Germany: n = 1606; Spain: n = 1647; Singapore: n = 1663) were affected by giving L1 English speaker status to standard L1 British and American English accents, compared with a typical Dutch English accent. We assessed the extent to which presumed nativeness impacted speaker evaluations (status, affect, dynamism), and the extent to which a speaker’s voice influenced speaker evaluations by analyzing listener responses to verbal and matched guises. The results showed that presumptions of a speaker’s nativeness significantly impacted speaker evaluations on all dimensions, and we therefore conclude that speaker evaluations are also based on listeners’ views on a speaker's nativeness. In addition, speaker evaluations were influenced by a speaker’s voice to such an extent that this can lead to significantly more positive/negative speaker evaluations of both L1 and L2 English speakers. Finally, this study confirms the relevance and main benefit of the matched-guise technique in accentedness research, compared with the verbal-guise technique, since the former successfully minimizes the actual impact of voice.
{"title":"Nativeness perceptions and speaker voice as predictors of (non-)native English speaker evaluations in four ELF contexts","authors":"Warda Nejjari, Roeland van Hout, Marinel Gerritsen, Brigitte Planken","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103737","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103737","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We investigated the extent to which responses (<em>N</em> = 6617) by four L2 English listener groups (The Netherlands: <em>n</em> = 1701; Germany: <em>n</em> = 1606; Spain: <em>n</em> = 1647; Singapore: <em>n</em> = 1663) were affected by giving L1 English speaker status to standard L1 British and American English accents, compared with a typical Dutch English accent. We assessed the extent to which <em>presumed nativeness</em> impacted speaker evaluations (status, affect, dynamism), and the extent to which a speaker’s <em>voice</em> influenced speaker evaluations by analyzing listener responses to verbal and matched guises. The results showed that presumptions of a speaker’s nativeness significantly impacted speaker evaluations on all dimensions, and we therefore conclude that speaker evaluations are also based on listeners’ views on a speaker's nativeness. In addition, speaker evaluations were influenced by a speaker’s voice to such an extent that this can lead to significantly more positive/negative speaker evaluations of both L1 and L2 English speakers. Finally, this study confirms the relevance and main benefit of the matched-guise technique in accentedness research, compared with the verbal-guise technique, since the former successfully minimizes the actual impact of voice.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"305 ","pages":"Article 103737"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0024384124000664/pdfft?md5=95ac09be50e6ee3ad28721883ed166f5&pid=1-s2.0-S0024384124000664-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140645942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-24DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103735
Daniel Karczewski , Alicja Zawistowska-Sadowska , Marcin Trojszczak
Normative generics, which are statements that can be seen as reflecting the social world, can be powerful tools by which parents convey norms to children. In this article, we explored how two interrelated issues, the perceived salience of gender stereotypes and the socially constructed system of patriarchy, could affect what parents communicate in the context of parent–child interactions that concern the breaching of a salient gender norm. By analyzing data from two perception studies, we found that some norms pertaining to hair length or table manners triggered a more frequent use of normative generics than other norms and that the individuals who espoused traditional gender values tended to prefer one mode of norm responsiveness (i.e., norm-following) and used more normative generics than those espousing nontraditional gender norms. In this way, the study contributed to our understanding of how rules of behavior shared by members of a given group, as well as beliefs in a system of hierarchical power, might favor the use of normative generics.
{"title":"Gender stereotypes, patriarchal beliefs, and normative generics: A survey-based measure of what Polish parents communicate in norm-breaching scenarios involving children","authors":"Daniel Karczewski , Alicja Zawistowska-Sadowska , Marcin Trojszczak","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103735","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103735","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Normative generics, which are statements that can be seen as reflecting the social world, can be powerful tools by which parents convey norms to children. In this article, we explored how two interrelated issues, the perceived salience of gender stereotypes and the socially constructed system of patriarchy, could affect what parents communicate in the context of parent–child interactions that concern the breaching of a salient gender norm. By analyzing data from two perception studies, we found that some norms pertaining to hair length or table manners triggered a more frequent use of normative generics than other norms and that the individuals who espoused traditional gender values tended to prefer one mode of norm responsiveness (i.e., norm-following) and used more normative generics than those espousing nontraditional gender norms. In this way, the study contributed to our understanding of how rules of behavior shared by members of a given group, as well as beliefs in a system of hierarchical power, might favor the use of normative generics.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"305 ","pages":"Article 103735"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140638465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-17DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103724
Estela Garcia-Alcaraz , Juana M. Liceras
Although bilingualism is encouraged and promoted among typically developing (TD) individuals, some countries continue to recommend monolingualism for non-TD individuals. This common practice seems unfounded because existing research investigating the effects of bilingualism on non-TD individuals has not revealed a detrimental effect of bilingualism. In this study, we analyzed the linguistic and metalinguistic abilities of individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS). To manipulate grammaticality and semantics, a grammaticality judgment task was created and administered in Spanish to eight Spanish monolingual speakers and seven Spanish–Catalan bilingual speakers, all with PWS. Similar results were obtained for linguistic and metalinguistic abilities in both groups, even if the bilingual speakers were Catalan dominant. Thus, these results not only support previous research within the field in not identifying a negative effect of bilingualism but also emphasize the fact that bilingual speakers can mirror monolingual speakers even in their “weaker” language.
{"title":"The linguistic and metalinguistic abilities of monolingual and bilingual speakers with Prader–Willi syndrome","authors":"Estela Garcia-Alcaraz , Juana M. Liceras","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103724","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103724","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although bilingualism is encouraged and promoted among typically developing (TD) individuals, some countries continue to recommend monolingualism for non-TD individuals. This common practice seems unfounded because existing research investigating the effects of bilingualism on non-TD individuals has not revealed a detrimental effect of bilingualism. In this study, we analyzed the linguistic and metalinguistic abilities of individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS). To manipulate grammaticality and semantics, a grammaticality judgment task was created and administered in Spanish to eight Spanish monolingual speakers and seven Spanish–Catalan bilingual speakers, all with PWS. Similar results were obtained for linguistic and metalinguistic abilities in both groups, even if the bilingual speakers were Catalan dominant. Thus, these results not only support previous research within the field in not identifying a negative effect of bilingualism but also emphasize the fact that bilingual speakers can mirror monolingual speakers even in their “weaker” language.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"304 ","pages":"Article 103724"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0024384124000536/pdfft?md5=4c04e9df2481cbe165bec247626d5dbe&pid=1-s2.0-S0024384124000536-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140558996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-04-12DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103710
Susana Rodríguez Rosique
This paper examines si-future exclamatives in Spanish, both synchronically and diachronically, with the aim not only of identifying their possible contexts of use but also of distinguishing their various semantic interpretations. In addition to permitting the redefinition of the space of insubordination and assessing the role of analogy in the continuing independence of the marker si, this proposal provides an explanation of Spanish mirative future as emerging from the emancipation of a previous structure, albeit that it retains the deictic value of morphological future –which becomes a functional feature in its new discursive meaning. From this perspective, the grammaticalization of morphological future in Spanish is not contemplated as a closed cycle, but rather as a still ongoing process. More broadly speaking, this paper helps outline the semantic array of mirative meanings through providing evidence about the type of mirativity that can be conveyed in Spanish.
本文从同步和非同步两个方面研究了西班牙语中的 si-future 感叹词,目的不仅在于确定其可能的使用语境,而且在于区分其各种语义解释。除了允许重新定义不服从的空间和评估类比在标记词 si 的持续独立性中所起的作用之外,该建议还提供了一种解释,即西班牙语镜像将来时是从先前结构的解放中产生的,尽管它保留了形态将来时的训示价值--这在其新的话语意义中成为一种功能特征。从这个角度看,形态未来语在西班牙语中的语法化并不是一个封闭的循环,而是一个仍在进行的过程。更广义地说,本文通过提供有关西班牙语中可以表达的 "镜像 "类型的证据,帮助勾勒出 "镜像 "意义的语义阵列。
{"title":"What is a morphological future doing in a si-clause? Traces of mirativity in Spanish","authors":"Susana Rodríguez Rosique","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103710","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103710","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This paper examines <em>si</em>-future exclamatives in Spanish, both synchronically and diachronically, with the aim not only of identifying their possible contexts of use but also of distinguishing their various semantic interpretations. In addition to permitting the redefinition of the space of insubordination and assessing the role of analogy in the continuing independence of the marker <em>si</em>, this proposal provides an explanation of Spanish mirative future as emerging from the emancipation of a previous structure, albeit that it retains the deictic value of morphological future –which becomes a functional feature in its new discursive meaning. From this perspective, the grammaticalization of morphological future in Spanish is not contemplated as a closed cycle, but rather as a still ongoing process. More broadly speaking, this paper helps outline the semantic array of mirative meanings through providing evidence about the type of mirativity that can be conveyed in Spanish.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"304 ","pages":"Article 103710"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0024384124000391/pdfft?md5=fe2a362c3ff1eac8651deed29819da6f&pid=1-s2.0-S0024384124000391-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140547081","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-03-28DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103716
Chen Zhao
Assuming the Strong Uniformity Principle, Miyagawa recently proposed a typology of languages based on distinct patterns of Feature Inheritance at Phase C. In this typology, Chinese is identified as a Category II language (with the δ-feature on C and the φ-feature on T). That is, Chinese is regarded as a non-discourse-configurational language similar to English. This paper aims to review Miyagawa’s discussion of Chinese and argue for its reclassification as a Category III language, that is, a discourse-configurational language like Spanish. I contend that Miyagawa’s discussion of Chinese only pertains to Aboutness-shift topic, a type of topic base-generated in the CP domain across languages; as a result, his conclusions are unsustainable. On the other hand, I show that phenomena like Object Preposing, forced A-focalization, and the distribution of Familiar and Contrastive topics in non-assertive embedded clauses provide compelling evidence that the δ-feature has lowered to T in Chinese. Finally, I address a potential counterargument presented by the Chinese wh-adverbial zenme ‘how come’. I argue that zenme is not base-generated in the CP periphery, but rather inserted in the TP layer.
{"title":"Chinese is a discourse-configurational language: Miyagawa’s typology revisited","authors":"Chen Zhao","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103716","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103716","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Assuming the Strong Uniformity Principle, Miyagawa recently proposed a typology of languages based on distinct patterns of Feature Inheritance at Phase C. In this typology, Chinese is identified as a Category II language (with the δ-feature on C and the φ-feature on T). That is, Chinese is regarded as a non-discourse-configurational language similar to English. This paper aims to review Miyagawa’s discussion of Chinese and argue for its reclassification as a Category III language, that is, a discourse-configurational language like Spanish. I contend that Miyagawa’s discussion of Chinese only pertains to Aboutness-shift topic, a type of topic base-generated in the CP domain across languages; as a result, his conclusions are unsustainable. On the other hand, I show that phenomena like Object Preposing, forced A-focalization, and the distribution of Familiar and Contrastive topics in non-assertive embedded clauses provide compelling evidence that the δ-feature has lowered to T in Chinese. Finally, I address a potential counterargument presented by the Chinese <em>wh</em>-adverbial <em>zenme</em> ‘how come’. I argue that <em>zenme</em> is not base-generated in the CP periphery, but rather inserted in the TP layer.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"304 ","pages":"Article 103716"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140309484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-03-27DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103714
Ming-Yu Tseng
This study investigates bidirectionality in figurative expressions by focusing on a pair of explicit bidirectional similes/metaphors in Chinese. This pair of similes/metaphors is also a saying: rénshēng rú xì, xì rú rénshēng (‘life is like a drama, a drama is like life’). The juxtaposition of bidirectional life–xì (‘drama’) similes – and the bidirectional pattern of metaphorical thought that motivates them – is rarely addressed in metaphor studies. By drawing on discourse examples in which the pair of metaphors is used, this paper examines how the bidirectional figurative expressions are interpreted in discourse and what light the pair of metaphors can shed on metaphorical bidirectionality. Building upon the recent discussion of metaphorical bidirectionality and extended conceptual metaphor theory, this paper develops an enriched understanding of bidirectionality, which is conceptualised as a symmetrical perspective that regards two subjects as interacting with one another on a more or less equal footing, or the principal subject and subsidiary subject as being reversible, and which exhibits unsettling metaphoricity in the literal–metaphorical continuum.
{"title":"What do Chinese bidirectional Life–Xì (‘Drama’) similes/metaphors tell us about metaphorical bidirectionality?","authors":"Ming-Yu Tseng","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103714","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103714","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigates bidirectionality in figurative expressions by focusing on a pair of explicit bidirectional similes/metaphors in Chinese. This pair of similes/metaphors is also a saying: <em>rénshēng rú xì, xì rú rénshēng</em> (‘life is like a drama, a drama is like life’). The juxtaposition of bidirectional <span>life</span>–<span>xì (‘</span>drama’) similes – and the bidirectional pattern of metaphorical thought that motivates them – is rarely addressed in metaphor studies. By drawing on discourse examples in which the pair of metaphors is used, this paper examines how the bidirectional figurative expressions are interpreted in discourse and what light the pair of metaphors can shed on metaphorical bidirectionality. Building upon the recent discussion of metaphorical bidirectionality and extended conceptual metaphor theory, this paper develops an enriched understanding of bidirectionality, which is conceptualised as a symmetrical perspective that regards two subjects as interacting with one another on a more or less equal footing, or the principal subject and subsidiary subject as being reversible, and which exhibits unsettling metaphoricity in the literal–metaphorical continuum.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"303 ","pages":"Article 103714"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140309270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-03-26DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103725
Chengtuan Li , Zhiwei Zhao , Jing Han
Drawing on Membership Categorization Analysis (MCA), this study investigates how and for what institutional goals, Chinese police officers, through doing self-disclosure, manipulate the membership categories of their own and the suspects and set up relational pairs in their interrogations of suspects. Analyzing 47 episodes of authentic police interrogations of suspects, we find that Chinese police officers’ self-disclosures coalesce around membership categorization in four ways: 1) the co-membership categorization to achieve account-challenging; 2) the co-membership categorization to realize persuasion; 3) the bonded relational pairing that entails a bond with suspects via standardized relational pairs to perform persuasion; 4) the conflictive relational pairing that showcases a conflict against suspects through relational pairs to accomplish accusation. The categorization practices underlying self-disclosures work to facilitate the institutional goals of persuasion and education (PE) of suspects in the Chinese context. Overall, these findings give a glimpse into the dynamic process of membership categorization between the Chinese police and the suspects during interrogations and offer a fresh perspective for analyzing self-disclosure practices in specific contexts.
{"title":"‘‘I’m the ghost hunter’’: Self-disclosure and its membership categorization in Chinese police interrogations of suspects","authors":"Chengtuan Li , Zhiwei Zhao , Jing Han","doi":"10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103725","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2024.103725","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Drawing on Membership Categorization Analysis (MCA), this study investigates how and for what institutional goals, Chinese police officers, through doing self-disclosure, manipulate the membership categories of their own and the suspects and set up relational pairs in their interrogations of suspects. Analyzing 47 episodes of authentic police interrogations of suspects, we find that Chinese police officers’ self-disclosures coalesce around membership categorization in four ways: 1) the co-membership categorization to achieve account-challenging; 2) the co-membership categorization to realize persuasion; 3) the bonded relational pairing that entails a bond with suspects via standardized relational pairs to perform persuasion; 4) the conflictive relational pairing that showcases a conflict against suspects through relational pairs to accomplish accusation. The categorization practices underlying self-disclosures work to facilitate the institutional goals of persuasion and education (PE) of suspects in the Chinese context. Overall, these findings give a glimpse into the dynamic process of membership categorization between the Chinese police and the suspects during interrogations and offer a fresh perspective for analyzing self-disclosure practices in specific contexts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":47955,"journal":{"name":"Lingua","volume":"304 ","pages":"Article 103725"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140296896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}