Pub Date : 2024-07-01DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101262
Ivan Axel Dounce, Félix Ramos
As humans, we have an excellent performance when perceiving the environment. In the artificial world, it is important for machines to perceive their environment so they can make correct decisions and act accordingly. An essential process to accomplish perception is to identify objects in a scene, but, as in reality, these objects can appear as ambiguous, and additionally, those objects are embedded into a particular scene. For our proposal, we created an architecture to identify ambiguous objects by using scene information to guide the identification process. The design is based on the human cortical systems that participate in object and scene recognition. In our study, we validate this proposal by analyzing a prior human experiment that demonstrates and quantifies the impact of scene information on ambiguous objects. Our findings demonstrate that employing the presented architecture on an object recognition task results in superior machine performance with familiar scenes, as opposed to unfamiliar or absent ones, consistent with human behavior.
{"title":"Biologically inspired architecture for the identification of ambiguous objects using scene associations","authors":"Ivan Axel Dounce, Félix Ramos","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101262","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101262","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As humans, we have an excellent performance when perceiving the environment. In the artificial world, it is important for machines to perceive their environment so they can make correct decisions and act accordingly. An essential process to accomplish perception is to identify objects in a scene, but, as in reality, these objects can appear as ambiguous, and additionally, those objects are embedded into a particular scene. For our proposal, we created an architecture to identify ambiguous objects by using scene information to guide the identification process. The design is based on the human cortical systems that participate in object and scene recognition. In our study, we validate this proposal by analyzing a prior human experiment that demonstrates and quantifies the impact of scene information on ambiguous objects. Our findings demonstrate that employing the presented architecture on an object recognition task results in superior machine performance with familiar scenes, as opposed to unfamiliar or absent ones, consistent with human behavior.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101262"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141543102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-26DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101261
Oleg Sychev
Automation of teaching people new skills requires modeling of human reasoning because human cognition involves active reasoning over the new subject domain to acquire skills that will later become automatic. The article presents Thought Process Trees — a language for modeling human reasoning that was created to facilitate the development of intelligent tutoring systems, which can perform the same reasoning that is expected of a student and find deficiencies in their line of thinking, providing explanatory messages and allowing them to learn from performance errors. The methodology of building trees which better reflect human learning is discussed, with examples of design choices during the modeling process and their consequences. The characteristics of educational modeling that impact building subject-domain models for intelligent tutoring systems are discussed. The trees were formalized and served as a basis for developing a framework for constructing intelligent tutoring systems. This significantly lowered the time required to build and debug a constraint-based subject-domain model. The framework has already been used to develop five intelligent tutoring systems and their prototypes and is being used to develop more of them.
{"title":"Educational models for cognition: Methodology of modeling intellectual skills for intelligent tutoring systems","authors":"Oleg Sychev","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101261","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Automation of teaching people new skills requires modeling of human reasoning because human cognition involves active reasoning over the new subject domain to acquire skills that will later become automatic. The article presents Thought Process Trees — a language for modeling human reasoning that was created to facilitate the development of intelligent tutoring systems, which can perform the same reasoning that is expected of a student and find deficiencies in their line of thinking, providing explanatory messages and allowing them to learn from performance errors. The methodology of building trees which better reflect human learning is discussed, with examples of design choices during the modeling process and their consequences. The characteristics of educational modeling that impact building subject-domain models for intelligent tutoring systems are discussed. The trees were formalized and served as a basis for developing a framework for constructing intelligent tutoring systems. This significantly lowered the time required to build and debug a constraint-based subject-domain model. The framework has already been used to develop five intelligent tutoring systems and their prototypes and is being used to develop more of them.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101261"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141543101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-18DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101257
Massimo Cossentino, Giovanni Pilato
In the last years, the System 1/System 2 cognitive architecture, proposed by psychologist Daniel Kahneman, raised the interest of many researchers in the field. System 1 is an intuitive, automatic, and fast-thinking system working effortlessly, without conscious effort. System 2 is a deliberate, analytical, and slower-thinking system employing conscious effort and attention. This work proposes an innovative approach that exploits techniques typical of information retrieval (the trie data structure) to efficiently encode the solutions’ repository at the border between System 2 and System 1. This repository stores the solutions (successful plans) the agent has already used and can re-enact to achieve the goals. System 2 conceives new plans and delegates System 1 to execute them. If the plan is successful (and so it becomes a solution), System 1 stores that in the repository to quickly retrieve any solution that may help fulfil the goals deliberated by System 2 in the future.
{"title":"Using a trie-based approach for storage and retrieval of goal-oriented plans in an S1/S2 cognitive architecture","authors":"Massimo Cossentino, Giovanni Pilato","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101257","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101257","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the last years, the System 1/System 2 cognitive architecture, proposed by psychologist Daniel Kahneman, raised the interest of many researchers in the field. <em>System 1</em> is an intuitive, automatic, and fast-thinking system working effortlessly, without conscious effort. <em>System 2</em> is a deliberate, analytical, and slower-thinking system employing conscious effort and attention. This work proposes an innovative approach that exploits techniques typical of information retrieval (the trie data structure) to efficiently encode the solutions’ repository at the border between System 2 and System 1. This repository stores the solutions (successful plans) the agent has already used and can re-enact to achieve the goals. System 2 conceives new plans and delegates System 1 to execute them. If the plan is successful (and so it becomes a solution), System 1 stores that in the repository to quickly retrieve any solution that may help fulfil the goals deliberated by System 2 in the future.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101257"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1389041724000512/pdfft?md5=90857dbbd82c5a305bde8800fccf4b48&pid=1-s2.0-S1389041724000512-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141543100","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-16DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101260
Christ Devia , Camilo Jara Do Nascimento , Samuel Madariaga , Pedro.E. Maldonado , Catalina Murúa , Rodrigo C. Vergara
This article presents a transdisciplinary analysis of the challenges in fusing neuroscience concepts with artificial intelligence (AI) to create AI systems inspired by biological cognition. We explore the structural and functional disparities between the neocortex’s canonical microcircuits and existing AI models, focusing on architectural differences, learning mechanisms, and energy efficiency. The discussion extends to adapting non-goal-oriented learning and dynamic neuronal connections from biological brains to enhance AI’s flexibility and efficiency. This work underscores the potential of neuroscientific insights to revolutionize AI development, advocating for a paradigm shift towards more adaptable and brain-like AI systems. We conclude that there is major room for bioinspiration by focusing on developing architecture, objective functions, and learning rules using a local instead of a global approach.
{"title":"Exploring biological challenges in building a thinking machine","authors":"Christ Devia , Camilo Jara Do Nascimento , Samuel Madariaga , Pedro.E. Maldonado , Catalina Murúa , Rodrigo C. Vergara","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101260","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101260","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This article presents a transdisciplinary analysis of the challenges in fusing neuroscience concepts with artificial intelligence (AI) to create AI systems inspired by biological cognition. We explore the structural and functional disparities between the neocortex’s canonical microcircuits and existing AI models, focusing on architectural differences, learning mechanisms, and energy efficiency. The discussion extends to adapting non-goal-oriented learning and dynamic neuronal connections from biological brains to enhance AI’s flexibility and efficiency. This work underscores the potential of neuroscientific insights to revolutionize AI development, advocating for a paradigm shift towards more adaptable and brain-like AI systems. We conclude that there is major room for bioinspiration by focusing on developing architecture, objective functions, and learning rules using a local instead of a global approach.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101260"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141412078","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-14DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101259
Shweta Singh , Vedant Ghatnekar , Sudaman Katti
The Human decision-making process works by recollecting past sequences of observations and using them to decide the best possible action in the present. These past sequences of observations are stored in a derived form which only includes important information the brain thinks might be useful in the future, while forgetting the rest. we propose an architecture that tries to mimic the human brain and improve the memory efficiency of transformers by using a modified TransformerXL architecture which uses Automatic Chunking which only attends to the relevant chunks in the transformer block. On top of this, we use ForgetSpan which is technique to remove memories that do not contribute to learning. We also theorize the technique of Similarity based forgetting to remove repetitive memories. We test our model in various tasks that test the abilities required to perform well in a human–robot collaboration scenario.
{"title":"Long horizon episodic decision making for cognitively inspired robots","authors":"Shweta Singh , Vedant Ghatnekar , Sudaman Katti","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101259","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101259","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The Human decision-making process works by recollecting past sequences of observations and using them to decide the best possible action in the present. These past sequences of observations are stored in a derived form which only includes important information the brain thinks might be useful in the future, while forgetting the rest. we propose an architecture that tries to mimic the human brain and improve the memory efficiency of transformers by using a modified TransformerXL architecture which uses Automatic Chunking which only attends to the relevant chunks in the transformer block. On top of this, we use ForgetSpan which is technique to remove memories that do not contribute to learning. We also theorize the technique of Similarity based forgetting to remove repetitive memories. We test our model in various tasks that test the abilities required to perform well in a human–robot collaboration scenario.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101259"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141482958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-14DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101258
Jiangli Duan , Guoyin Wang , Xin Hu , Qun Liu , Qin Jiang , Huamin Zhu
As part of cognitive intelligence, concept cognition for knowledge graphs aims to clearly grasp the typical characteristics of the things referred to by the concept, which can provide prior knowledge for machine understanding and thinking. Different from concept learning and formal concept analysis that learn new concepts from data and the general decision rule that comes from an independent decision table, this paper cognizes an existing concept by decision rules that come from multiple granularities. Specifically, 1) concept cognition for knowledge graphs is realized from the perspective of mining multi-granularity decision rule. 2) Decision tables corresponding to four granularities form a multi-granularity decision table group, and then the result from coarser granularity can guide and help obtaining the result from finer granularity. 3) We propose a framework for mining multi-granularity decision rules, which involves going from a multi-granularity decision table group to the frequent maximal attribute patterns to the decision rules to the credible decision rules. Finally, we verified effectiveness of dividing positive and negative data, monotonicity of attribute patterns in a multi-granularity decision table group, and downward monotonicity of credibility, and observed the impact of the parameter min_cov and min_conf on execution times.
{"title":"Concept cognition for knowledge graphs: Mining multi-granularity decision rule","authors":"Jiangli Duan , Guoyin Wang , Xin Hu , Qun Liu , Qin Jiang , Huamin Zhu","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101258","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101258","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>As part of cognitive intelligence, concept cognition for knowledge graphs aims to clearly grasp the typical characteristics of the things referred to by the concept, which can provide prior knowledge for machine understanding and thinking. Different from concept learning and formal concept analysis that learn new concepts from data and the general decision rule that comes from an independent decision table, this paper cognizes an existing concept by decision rules that come from multiple granularities. Specifically, 1) concept cognition for knowledge graphs is realized from the perspective of mining multi-granularity decision rule. 2) Decision tables corresponding to four granularities form a multi-granularity decision table group, and then the result from coarser granularity can guide and help obtaining the result from finer granularity. 3) We propose a framework for mining multi-granularity decision rules, which involves going from a multi-granularity decision table group to the frequent maximal attribute patterns to the decision rules to the credible decision rules. Finally, we verified effectiveness of dividing positive and negative data, monotonicity of attribute patterns in a multi-granularity decision table group, and downward monotonicity of credibility, and observed the impact of the parameter <em>min_cov</em> and <em>min_conf</em> on execution times.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101258"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141407118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-13DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101256
Rubén Torres Agustín , Pablo González Francisco , Lilia Mestas Hernández , Martha Alejandra Gómez-López , Francisco Abelardo Robles Aguirre
Music has been used to express and communicate emotional states through its different dimensions such as tone, rhythm, melody, and harmony. Consonant harmonies consistently are rated as pleasant whereas dissonant are considered unpleasant. The aim of this study was to explore the effect of consonant and dissonant musical harmonies used as prime on the emotional classification of images, as indexed by event-related potentials. Thirty volunteers (ages 21–27, 50 % women) were presented with a task consisting of 4 musical intervals in the C major scale, divided into consonant and dissonant harmonies, followed by 180 positive, negative, or neutral images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Participants had to rate the images as pleasant or unpleasant. We found a bias effect on negative images rated as positive when preceded by a consonant musical interval. A N200 component, non-sensible to the valence of the images, was found. On the other hand, a significant difference was found in the amplitude of the P300 component, with a greater amplitude in the consonant-positive images condition compared to the dissonant-positive images. Lastly, a late positivity component around 500–700 ms was found in both negative conditions dissonant and consonant, but with a larger amplitude for the consonant condition when followed by a negative image. These results indicate that additionally to the P300 processing the relevance of the stimulus there are processes like recognition memory involved. As part of the novelty effect this late positive activity may also be related to the emotional content integration of the relevant stimulus.
{"title":"Musical harmonies and its relationship with emotional processing: An ERP study in young adults","authors":"Rubén Torres Agustín , Pablo González Francisco , Lilia Mestas Hernández , Martha Alejandra Gómez-López , Francisco Abelardo Robles Aguirre","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101256","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101256","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Music has been used to express and communicate emotional states through its different dimensions such as tone, rhythm, melody, and harmony. Consonant harmonies consistently are rated as pleasant whereas dissonant are considered unpleasant. The aim of this study was to explore the effect of consonant and dissonant musical harmonies used as prime on the emotional classification of images, as indexed by event-related potentials. Thirty volunteers (ages 21–27, 50 % women) were presented with a task consisting of 4 musical intervals in the C major scale, divided into consonant and dissonant harmonies, followed by 180 positive, negative, or neutral images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Participants had to rate the images as pleasant or unpleasant. We found a bias effect on negative images rated as positive when preceded by a consonant musical interval. A N200 component, non-sensible to the valence of the images, was found. On the other hand, a significant difference was found in the amplitude of the P300 component, with a greater amplitude in the consonant-positive images condition compared to the dissonant-positive images. Lastly, a late positivity component around 500–700 ms was found in both negative conditions dissonant and consonant, but with a larger amplitude for the consonant condition when followed by a negative image. These results indicate that additionally to the P300 processing the relevance of the stimulus there are processes like recognition memory involved. As part of the novelty effect this late positive activity may also be related to the emotional content integration of the relevant stimulus.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101256"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141404873","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-06-04DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101255
Serge Sonfack Sounchio, Laurent Geneste, Bernard Kamsu Foguem
The hypothesis-driven methodology is a cognitive activity used in expertise processes to solve problems with limited knowledge and understanding. Although some organizations have standardized this approach to guide humans in carrying out expertise in enterprises, it lacks appropriate tools to assist experts in carrying out this cognitive activity, tracking understanding, or capturing the reasoning steps and the knowledge produced during the process.
To acquire, share and reuse experts’ knowledge applied during expertise processes while assisting humans in bringing understanding to complex problems, this study introduces a human–machine collaborative framework that formalizes experts’ knowledge from the hypothesis-driven methodology described in the France standard NF X50-110 of “Quality of expertise activity”. This framework utilizes Hypothesis Theory extended with qualitative doubt and a systematic reasoning process to generate a hypothesis exploratory graph (HEG).
The proposed approach makes it easier to carry out expertise processes through a human–machine collaboration, offers a means to share and reuse knowledge from expertise, and provides expertise processes evaluation mechanisms. Furthermore, an experiment conducted on a use-case of expertise process verifies the feasibility and effectiveness of the approach.
{"title":"A hypotheses-driven framework for human–machine expertise process","authors":"Serge Sonfack Sounchio, Laurent Geneste, Bernard Kamsu Foguem","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101255","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101255","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The hypothesis-driven methodology is a cognitive activity used in expertise processes to solve problems with limited knowledge and understanding. Although some organizations have standardized this approach to guide humans in carrying out expertise in enterprises, it lacks appropriate tools to assist experts in carrying out this cognitive activity, tracking understanding, or capturing the reasoning steps and the knowledge produced during the process.</p><p>To acquire, share and reuse experts’ knowledge applied during expertise processes while assisting humans in bringing understanding to complex problems, this study introduces a human–machine collaborative framework that formalizes experts’ knowledge from the hypothesis-driven methodology described in the France standard NF X50-110 of “Quality of expertise activity”. This framework utilizes Hypothesis Theory extended with qualitative doubt and a systematic reasoning process to generate a hypothesis exploratory graph (HEG).</p><p>The proposed approach makes it easier to carry out expertise processes through a human–machine collaboration, offers a means to share and reuse knowledge from expertise, and provides expertise processes evaluation mechanisms. Furthermore, an experiment conducted on a use-case of expertise process verifies the feasibility and effectiveness of the approach.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101255"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-06-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1389041724000494/pdfft?md5=9c52e6fa4afbcba466c874d4febe947f&pid=1-s2.0-S1389041724000494-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141276501","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-06DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101246
Deep learning approaches to mind, brain, and behavior raise new philosophical and methodological questions about the nature of artificial intelligence (AI) and its relationship to biological cognitive systems. The articles in this Special Issue combine insights, results and methodologies from philosophy, psychology, AI, neuroscience, linguistics, and cognitive science more generally, to explore some of those questions, including the relation between deep learning models and the brain, the testability, transparency and explanatory power of deep learning models, and their abilities for inductive reasoning, language processing and semantic understanding. By engaging with these foundational questions, the Special Issue as a whole contributes to illuminate deep learning, illustrating the need for, and fruitfulness of, interdisciplinary perspectives in cognitive systems research.
{"title":"Foundations of Deep Learning. An introduction to the Special Issue","authors":"","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101246","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101246","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Deep learning approaches to mind, brain, and behavior raise new philosophical and methodological questions about the nature of artificial intelligence (AI) and its relationship to biological cognitive systems. The articles in this Special Issue combine insights, results and methodologies from philosophy, psychology, AI, neuroscience, linguistics, and cognitive science more generally, to explore some of those questions, including the relation between deep learning models and the brain, the testability, transparency and explanatory power of deep learning models, and their abilities for inductive reasoning, language processing and semantic understanding. By engaging with these foundational questions, the Special Issue as a whole contributes to illuminate deep learning, illustrating the need for, and fruitfulness of, interdisciplinary perspectives in cognitive systems research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"87 ","pages":"Article 101246"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141040850","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2024-05-06DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101243
Carl O. Retzlaff , Alessa Angerschmid , Anna Saranti , David Schneeberger , Richard Röttger , Heimo Müller , Andreas Holzinger
The growing field of explainable Artificial Intelligence (xAI) has given rise to a multitude of techniques and methodologies, yet this expansion has created a growing gap between existing xAI approaches and their practical application. This poses a considerable obstacle for data scientists striving to identify the optimal xAI technique for their needs. To address this problem, our study presents a customized decision support framework to aid data scientists in choosing a suitable xAI approach for their use-case. Drawing from a literature survey and insights from interviews with five experienced data scientists, we introduce a decision tree based on the trade-offs inherent in various xAI approaches, guiding the selection between six commonly used xAI tools. Our work critically examines six prevalent ante-hoc and post-hoc xAI methods, assessing their applicability in real-world contexts through expert interviews. The aim is to equip data scientists and policymakers with the capacity to select xAI methods that not only demystify the decision-making process, but also enrich user understanding and interpretation, ultimately advancing the application of xAI in practical settings.
{"title":"Post-hoc vs ante-hoc explanations: xAI design guidelines for data scientists","authors":"Carl O. Retzlaff , Alessa Angerschmid , Anna Saranti , David Schneeberger , Richard Röttger , Heimo Müller , Andreas Holzinger","doi":"10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101243","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2024.101243","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The growing field of explainable Artificial Intelligence (xAI) has given rise to a multitude of techniques and methodologies, yet this expansion has created a growing gap between existing xAI approaches and their practical application. This poses a considerable obstacle for data scientists striving to identify the optimal xAI technique for their needs. To address this problem, our study presents a customized decision support framework to aid data scientists in choosing a suitable xAI approach for their use-case. Drawing from a literature survey and insights from interviews with five experienced data scientists, we introduce a decision tree based on the trade-offs inherent in various xAI approaches, guiding the selection between six commonly used xAI tools. Our work critically examines six prevalent ante-hoc and post-hoc xAI methods, assessing their applicability in real-world contexts through expert interviews. The aim is to equip data scientists and policymakers with the capacity to select xAI methods that not only demystify the decision-making process, but also enrich user understanding and interpretation, ultimately advancing the application of xAI in practical settings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":55242,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Systems Research","volume":"86 ","pages":"Article 101243"},"PeriodicalIF":3.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140880349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}